ArticlePDF Available

Maternal sensitivity predicts anterior hippocampal functional networks in early childhood

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Maternal care influences child hippocampal development. The hippocampus is functionally organized along an anterior–posterior axis. Little is known with regards to the extent maternal care shapes offspring anterior and posterior hippocampal (aHPC, pHPC) functional networks. This study examined maternal behavior, especially maternal sensitivity, at 6 months postpartum in relation to aHPC and pHPC functional networks of children at age 4 and 6 years. Maternal sensitivity was assessed at 6 months via the “Maternal Behavior Q Sort (MBQS) mini for video”. Subsequently, 61 and 76 children underwent resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging (rs-fMRI), respectively, at 4 and 6 years of age. We found that maternal sensitivity assessed at 6 months postpartum was associated with the right aHPC functional networks in children at both 4 and 6 years of age. At age 4 years, maternal sensitivity was associated positively with the right aHPC’s functional connectivity with the sensorimotor network and negatively with the aHPC’s functional connectivity with the top–down cognitive control network. At 6 years of age, maternal sensitivity was linked positively with the right aHPC’s functional connectivity with the visual-processing network. Our findings suggested that maternal sensitivity in infancy has a long-term impact on the anterior hippocampal functional network in preschool children, implicating a potential role of maternal care in shaping child brain development in early life.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Vol.:(0123456789)
1 3
Brain Structure and Function (2019) 224:1885–1895
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-019-01882-0
ORIGINAL ARTICLE
Maternal sensitivity predicts anterior hippocampal functional
networks inearly childhood
QiangWang1· HanZhang1· Chong‑YawWee1· AnnieLee1· JoannS.Poh1· Yap‑SengChong2,3· KokHianTan4·
PeterD.Gluckman2,5· FabianYap8· MarielleV.Fortier6· AnneRifkin‑Graboi7· AnqiQiu1
Received: 8 January 2019 / Accepted: 19 April 2019 / Published online: 4 May 2019
© Springer-Verlag GmbH Germany, part of Springer Nature 2019
Abstract
Maternal care influences child hippocampal development. The hippocampus is functionally organized along an anterior–
posterior axis. Little is known with regards to the extent maternal care shapes offspring anterior and posterior hippocampal
(aHPC, pHPC) functional networks. This study examined maternal behavior, especially maternal sensitivity, at 6months
postpartum in relation to aHPC and pHPC functional networks of children at age 4 and 6years. Maternal sensitivity was
assessed at 6months via the “Maternal Behavior Q Sort (MBQS) mini for video”. Subsequently, 61 and 76 children under-
went resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging (rs-fMRI), respectively, at 4 and 6years of age. We found that
maternal sensitivity assessed at 6months postpartum was associated with theright aHPC functional networks in children
at both 4 and 6years of age. At age 4years, maternal sensitivity was associated positively with the right aHPC’s functional
connectivity with the sensorimotor network and negatively with the aHPC’s functional connectivity with the top–down
cognitive control network. At 6years of age, maternal sensitivity was linked positively with the right aHPC’s functional
connectivity with the visual-processing network. Our findings suggested that maternal sensitivity in infancy has a long-term
impact on the anterior hippocampal functional network in preschool children, implicating a potential role of maternal care
in shaping child brain development in early life.
Keywords Resting-state fMRI· Maternal sensitivity· Anterior hippocampus· Posterior hippocampus· Functional
networks
Electronic supplementary material The online version of this
article (https ://doi.org/10.1007/s0042 9-019-01882 -0) contains
supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.
* Anqi Qiu
bieqa@nus.edu.sg
1 Department ofBiomedical Engineering andClinical
Imaging Research Center, National University ofSingapore,
4 Engineering Drive 3 Block E4 #04-08, Singapore117583,
Singapore
2 Singapore Institute forClinical Sciences, Singapore117609,
Singapore
3 Department ofObstetrics andGynaecology, Yong Loo
Lin School ofMedicine, National University ofSingapore,
National University Health System, Singapore, Singapore
4 Department ofMaternal–Fetal Medicine, KK
Women’s andChildren’s Hospital, Singapore (KKH),
Singapore229899, Singapore
5 Liggins Institute, University ofAuckland, Auckland1142,
NewZealand
6 Department ofDiagnostic andInterventional Imaging,
KK Women’s andChildren’s Hospital, Singapore (KKH),
Singapore229899, Singapore
7 Office ofEducation Research, Centre forResearch inChild
Development, Office ofEducation Research, National
Institute ofEducation, Nanyang Technical University,
Singapore, Singapore
8 Department ofPaediatrics, KK Women’s andChildren’s
Hospital, Singapore229899, Singapore
1886 Brain Structure and Function (2019) 224:1885–1895
1 3
Introduction
Maternal sensitivity has been defined as the ability to rec-
ognize and respond both effectively and promptly to the
distress and needs of one’s child (Ainsworth etal. 1978). It
is widely associated with infant’s early linguistic (Paavola
etal. 2006), cognitive (Frick etal. 2018), and self-regula-
tion development (Ispa etal. 2017; Frick etal. 2018), as
well as school-age performance (Treyvaud etal. 2016).
Maternal sensitivity is also considered protective, buffer-
ing influences of early adversity on child behavioral and
cognitive development (Faure etal. 2017; Drury 2012).
Currently, much attention has been devoted to the relation-
ships between maternal sensitivity and child cognition and
behavior. Only a few studies (Rifkin-Graboi etal. 2015a;
Wen etal. 2017b) have begun to explore maternal sensi-
tivity’s influence upon functional brain networks in early
childhood, which may, perhaps in turn, bias subsequent
offspring sensory information and cognitive processing.
Converging evidence has documented that the broader
concept of maternal care impacts hippocampal gluco-
corticoid receptors (Liu etal. 1997), hippocampal tran-
scriptome (Weaver etal. 2006), hippocampal plasticity
(Champagne etal. 2008) and function (Bagot etal. 2012;
Nguyen etal. 2015) in animal models. In line with these
findings, human-imaging studies have found that mater-
nal care associates with a larger hippocampal volume in
the preschool period (Luby etal. 2016b). Also, past work
with 6-month-old infants demonstrates the relationship of
maternal sensitivity with the hippocampal volume, as well
as hippocampal functional connectivity with brain regions
involved in cognitive flexibility, such as the dorsolateral
prefrontal cortex (dlPFC), and autobiographical memory,
such as the lingual gyrus (Rifkin-Graboi etal. 2015a).
Nevertheless, some imaging studies revealed negative
association or no association between the aspect of posi-
tive parenting (Rao etal. 2010; Bernier etal. 2019) and
the hippocampal volume in early childhood (Whittle etal.
2014). The specificity of relations between the caregiving
environment and brain development may be influenced by
the timing of exposure and the age of MRI assessment.
One proposed mechanism influencing such neural
changes involves the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal
(HPA) axis and stress responsiveness. That is, low mater-
nal sensitivity may be a salient source of stress in off-
spring during early development, and as such may alter
HPA reactivity and eventually children’s emotional experi-
ence and neurocognitive development (Blair etal. 2006).
Importantly, the hippocampus is one of the core brain
regions involved in stress responsiveness and regulation
(Herman etal. 2005). It is also essential for autobiographi-
cal memory and thinking about the future—two processes
critical to the building of cognitive-emotional schemas
that may guide daily life and social relationships (Cabeza
and St Jacques 2007; Hassabis etal. 2007).
The hippocampus is thought to be functionally organ-
ized along an anterior–posterior axis (Poppenk etal. 2013).
A few studies suggested that the anterior hippocampus is
involved in the non-emotional processes such as spatial
memory (Strange etal. 2014; Zeidman and Maguire 2016;
Sapolsky etal. 1985). Moreover, the posterior hippocampus
is engaged to the stress-related traits such as anxiety and
depression (Satpute etal. 2012) and chronic stress (Sapolsky
etal. 1985), as well as is associated with risk of anxiety and
depression (de Geus etal. 2007). Nevertheless, there is a
consensus that the anterior portion of the hippocampus is
thought to reflect motivational processing and information
encoding in episodic memory, whereas the posterior portion
of the hippocampus is responsible for retrieval processing
in episodic memory and spatial cognition (Poppenk etal.
2013; Nadel etal. 2012). In addition, animal and human
studies show a specific role of the anterior hippocampus
in anxiety-related behaviors (Bannerman etal. 2004; Sat-
pute etal. 2012) and stress-related processing via its close
connections with other subcortical structures relevant to the
HPA axis (Bannerman etal. 2004; Fanselow and Dong 2010;
Mahar etal. 2014). This may suggest that stress related to
the caregiving environment has a larger impact upon the
anterior, rather than the posterior hippocampus. Moreover,
young adults with a history of caregiving deprivation and
emotional neglect were found to exhibit greater anterior, but
not posterior, hippocampal activation during processing of
threatening information such as fearful faces (Maheu etal.
2010). In line with these findings, Fanselow and Dong have
reviewed research indicating that the anterior hippocampus
and its neural connectivity are involved in stress, emotion,
and affect from the behavioral, anatomical, and genetic
perspectives. Hence, we hypothesized that the anterior hip-
pocampal circuitry might be predominantly influenced by
maternal care in early childhood. However, it is unclear
which specific anterior hippocampal circuitry could be
influenced by maternal care. Given the previous findings
on the emphasis of hippocampal-sensory and cognitive net-
works in relation to maternal sensitivity in infants (Rifkin-
Graboi etal. 2015a), we would expect that these networks
may continue being influenced by maternal sensitivity in
early childhood.
In this study, we investigated the above hypothesis and
examined the extent to which maternal sensitivity is asso-
ciated with anterior and posterior hippocampal functional
networks in preschool children at 4 and 6years of age using
resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging (rs-
fMRI). We studied preschool children partly because of the
rapid development of the brain during this period and the
feasibility of imaging children at this young age. Moreover,
1887Brain Structure and Function (2019) 224:1885–1895
1 3
a previous study also suggested that preschool is a sensitive
period for the influence of maternal support on hippocampal
development (Luby etal. 2016a). Moreover, the associa-
tions of maternal behaviors/adversity with child behaviors
and neurodevelopment are sex dependent. Maternal cortisol
during pregnancy was associated with affective problems
and a larger amygdala volume in girls but not in boys (Buss
etal. 2012). Maternal depressive symptoms were linked
with more internalizing behavioral problems in girls than
in boys (Essex etal. 2003) and were associated with the
amygdala structural and functional development in 4-year-
old girls but not boys (Wen etal. 2017a; Soe etal. 2016).
Hence, we examined the interactive effect of sex-by-mater-
nal sensitivity on the anterior and posterior hippocampal
functional networks. We then utilized a reduced model to
independently estimate maternal sensitivity’s influence upon
anterior and posterior hippocampal functional connectivity
when no interaction effects were observed. We expected that
the association of maternal sensitivity with the hippocampal
functional networks might be sex dependent.
Materials andmethods
Participants
This study was approved by the National Healthcare Group
Domain Specific Review Board (NHGDSRB) and the Sing
Health Centralized Institutional Review Board (CIRB).
Written informed consent was obtained from mothers and
the oral informed consent was obtained from children prior
to participation.
Three hundred and forty-two and 398 mother–child dyads
who participated in the prospective Growing Up in Singa-
pore Towards healthy Outcomes (GUSTO) birth cohort
study were recruited for neuroimaging when children were
4 years and 6 years of age, respectively. The details of the
GUSTO cohort can be found in our previous papers (Soh
etal. 2012).
Maternal education level and maternal ethnicity were
obtained from survey questionnaires conducted as part of a
scheduled appointment during the 26th week of pregnancy.
Birth outcomes (gestational age, birth weight, Appearance,
Pulse, Grimace, Activity, and Respiration (APGAR) score
and sex) and pregnancy measures were obtained from hos-
pital records. This study only included children with ges-
tational age 34weeks, birth weight 2kg and a 5-min
APGAR score 9 to avoid potential influences of pregnancy
outcomes upon brain development. Our inclusion criteria
were matched with previously reported criteria (Buss etal.
2012).
Of the 342 subjects who underwent MRI at 4years of
age, 78 subjects had unusable T1 data due to unsatisfactory
image quality, 4 did not meet the inclusion criteria, 119
mothers did not have maternal sensitivity data, 10 mothers
had unusable maternal sensitivity data (i.e., data lost (n = 3),
faulty audio (n = 5), view issue (n = 1), and language issue
(n = 1)), 30 mothers of infants did not complete depression
questionnaires (i.e., EPDS), 2 mothers did not complete
demographic information questionnaires (i.e., maternal edu-
cation) and 38 subjects had large head motion of rs-fMRI
(maximal framewise displacement, FD > 0.5mm). Hence,
the 4-year-old sample in this study included 61 subjects (33
girls and 28 boys).
Of the 398 subjects who underwent MRI at 6years of
age, 81 subjects had unusable T1 data due to unsatisfac-
tory image quality, 7 did not meet the inclusion criteria, 149
mothers did not have maternal sensitivity data, 16 moth-
ers had unusable maternal sensitivity data (i.e., data lost
(n = 3), faulty audio (n = 7), view issue (n = 3), procedural
inconsistency (n = 2), and father–child interaction (n = 1)),
28 mothers of infants did not complete depression question-
naires (i.e., EPDS), 2 mothers did not complete demographic
information questionnaires (i.e., maternal education) and 39
subjects had large head motion of rs-fMRI (FD > 0.5mm).
Hence, the 6-year-old sample in this study included 76 sub-
jects (46 girls and 30 boys). Table1 lists the demographic
information of the two samples that were used in this study.
Maternal sensitivity
A 15-min mother–child interaction was recorded as part of
a 3-h laboratory visit when infants were 6months of age
2weeks). The mother was asked to “interact or play” with
her 6-month-old infant “as she normally would at home”.
The room was equipped with a foldable chair, highchair,
and a mat, but no toys for the first 5min. After 5min, a
standard set of attractive toys and books was brought into the
room. Maternal sensitivity was assessed using the Revised
Mini-A short form of the Maternal Behavioral Q-Sort-V
(Mini-MBQS-V) (Tarabulsy etal. 2009). The Mini-MBQS-
V consists of 25 items, each representing different possible
aspects of sensitive, and inversely, insensitive, maternal
behavior during interaction with an infant. Coders sort the
25 items into piles of 5, ranging from 1 being “least like
the mother”, to 5 being “most like the mother.” Ratings are
then correlated with that of a theoretically constructed pro-
totypical sensitive mother to derive the global sensitivity
score, ranging from − 1 (very much unlike a prototypical
sensitive mother) to 1 (very much similar to a prototypical
sensitive mother). For example, if the mother’s behavior is
very similar to a “prototypically sensitive mother”, coders
might assign values of “5” to cards such as: “Mother builds
on the focus of the baby’s attention” and “Mother responds
to the baby’s distress and non-distress signals even when
engaged in some other activity”. Likewise, when viewing
1888 Brain Structure and Function (2019) 224:1885–1895
1 3
a mother who is very similar to a prototypically sensitive
mother, coders might assign values of “1” to cards describ-
ing insensitive behavior such as, “Mother tends to tune out
and not notice the infant’s bids for attention” and “The con-
tent and pace of the interaction is set by the mother rather
than the baby’s response”. Three Southeast Asian coders
scored the larger GUSTO cohort cases, and the two of whom
who scored the majority of cases were directly trained by the
developers of the Mini-MQS-V coding system. Together, the
local coders were fluent in both English and the predominant
mother tongue languages of Singapore (i.e., Malay, Manda-
rin, and/or Tamil). Training included the scoring of West-
ern and Singaporean tapes. Reliability for MBQS sensitivity
was assessed between the first two coders across 59 cases
(roughly 15% of the GUSTO sample), and between Coder
Three and Coder One and Coder Two, respectively, across
35 and 31 tapes. The Absolute Intraclass Correlation Coef-
ficient (ICC) Single Measures across all three coders for
sensitivity equaled 0.720, and was 0.861 between Coders
One and Two.
Maternal depression scales
We included the scale of maternal depressive symptoms as
a confounding variable as it is negatively associated with
maternal sensitivity (Crockenberg and Leerkes 2003).
Maternal depressive symptoms were assessed using the
Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale (EPDS) at 3months
postpartum. The EPDS is a widely used 10-item self-report
scale designed as a screening instrument for maternal
postnatal depression and valid for use in the prenatal and
early postnatal time points (Bergink etal. 2011). Each EPDS
item is scored on a four-point scale (0–3), and items three
and five-through-ten are reverse scored. All item points
are summed for a total score. Higher EPDS scores indicate
higher levels of depressive symptomatology.
MRI acquisition andpreprocessing
Children underwent MRI scans at age of 4.5 years
1months) and 6years (± 2months) using a 3T Siemens
Skyra scanner with a 32-channel head coil at KK Women’s
and Children’s hospital. The image protocols were: (1)
high-resolution isotropic T1-weighted Magnetization Pre-
pared Rapid Gradient Recalled Echo (MPRAGE; 192 slices,
1mm thickness, in-plane resolution 1mm, sagittal acqui-
sition, field of view 192 × 192mm2, matr ix = 192 × 192,
repetition time = 2000ms, echo time = 2.08ms, inversion
time = 877ms, flip angle = 9°, scanning time = 3.5min); (2)
isotropic axial rs-fMRI protocol (single-shot echo-planar
imaging; 48 slices with 3mm slice thickness, no inter-slice
gaps, matrix = 64 × 64, field of view = 192 × 192mm2, rep-
etition time = 2660ms, echo time = 27ms, flip angle = 90°,
scan time of the first run = 5.27min, scan time of the sec-
ond run = 3.19min). The children were required to close
their eyes during the rs-fMRI scan. The practical scanning
procedure was detailed in the Supplementary of (Wen etal.
2017a). Only the first run of rs-fMRI was used in this study.
The image quality was verified immediately after the
acquisition through visual inspection when children were
Table 1 Demographics
SD standard deviation, EPDS Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale, APGAR appearance, pulse, grimace, activity, and respiration
Measure 4-year-old sample (N = 61) 6-year-old sample (N = 76)
APGAR score, mean ± SD (min–max) 9.02 ± 0.13 (9–10) 9.0 ± 0.0 (9–9)
Gestational age (week), mean ± SD (min–max) 38.76 ± 1.35 (34.8–40.6) 39.12 ± 1.11 (34.8–41.1)
Birth weight (g), mean ± SD (min–max) 3133.4 ± 434.8 (2265–4390) 3118.2 ± 447.2 (2265–4390)
Sex, male/female 33/28 46/30
Age (year), mean ± SD (min–max) 4.57 ± 0.08 (4.36–4.75) 6.03 ± 0.13 (5.83–6.61)
Maternal sensitivity score, mean ± SD (min–max) 0.25 ± 0.49 (− 0.68–0.85) 0.21 ± 0.44 (− 0.68–0.81)
3-month EPDS score, mean ± SD (min–max) 8.46 ± 8.56 (0–38) 7.53 ± 6.71 (0–32)
Maternal ethnicity, %
Chinese 42.6 40.8
Malay 36.1 36.8
Indian 21.3 22.4
Maternal education, %
Primary school 4.9 4.0
Secondary school 29.5 27.6
Pre-university, diploma or technical course 36.1 36.8
University undergraduate level 26.2 30.3
Above university undergraduate level 3.3 1.3
1889Brain Structure and Function (2019) 224:1885–1895
1 3
still in the scanner. A scan was repeated when the ring
artifact on T1-weighted images was large. The image
was removed from the study if no acceptable image was
acquired after three repetitions.
Structural MRI FreeSurfer was used to segment brain images
into three tissue types, gray matter (GM), white matter
(WM), and cerebrospinal fluid (CSF). Post-processing qual-
ity checks were conducted according to instructions on https
://surfe r.nmr.mgh.harva rd.edu/fswik i/FsTut orial /Troub lesho
oting Data. Non-linear image normalization was achieved
by aligning individual T1-weighted MRI images to the JHU
atlas (Zhang etal. 2014; Mori etal. 2008) via large deforma-
tion diffeomorphic metric mapping (LDDMM) (Tan and Qiu
2016; Du etal. 2011; Zhong etal. 2010). Visual inspection
was conducted to detect any obvious mapping errors.
Rs-fMRI Rs-fMRI data that had more than 10 volumes in a
row with large motion or a checkered-board image appear-
ance were discarded from any further analysis. FSL was
used to process the rs-fMRI scan for slice time correction,
motion correction, skull stripping, and intensity normaliza-
tion. We computed framewise displacement (FD) of a time
series based on the definition given in (Power etal. 2012) to
quantify head motion. In brief, FD was defined as the sum of
the absolute values of the derivatives of the six realignment
parameters. Rotational displacements are converted from
degrees to millimeters by calculating displacement on the
surface of a sphere of radius 50mm (Power etal. 2012). We
excluded rs-fMRI data if one or multiple volumes had frame-
wise displacement (FD) greater than 0.5mm. The mean and
standard deviation values of the maximal FD among the sub-
jects included in this study were 0.181mm and 0.136mm
in the 4-year-old sample and 0.198mm and 0.134mm in
the 6-year-old sample; the range was from 0.033mm to
0.496mm in the 4-year-old sample and from 0.033mm to
0.481mm in the 6-year-old sample. Figure S1 in the Sup-
plementary Material shows the distributions of max FD and
FD of each rs-fMRI volume over all the subjects at 4 and
6years, as well as FD across the time series in subjects
with the lowest and highest motion at 4 and 6years. Linear
regression analysis was performed to partial out six motion
parameters (three translations and three rotation parameters),
global signal, WM and CSF signals. Global signal regres-
sion was carried out to eliminate artifactual variance due to
head motion, known to be a problem in pediatric popula-
tions (Power etal. 2014). Band-pass filtering (0.01–0.08Hz)
was then applied. For each subject, the mean functional
volume was aligned to the corresponding anatomical image
via rigid body alignment. The functional data were finally
transformed to the JHU atlas space via LDDMM obtained
based on the T1-weighted MRI.
Anterior andposterior hippocampal functional
networks
The hippocampus was defined using the Harvard–Oxford
subcortical atlas (Desikan etal. 2006) from the FSL Soft-
ware Library (Smith etal. 2004). Next, the left and right
hippocampi were vertically divided into the anterior (aHPC)
and posterior (pHPC) segments at Y = − 21mm in the MNI
space (Poppenk etal. 2013). As illustrated in Fig.1, we
discarded 2 coronal slices between the anterior and poste-
rior segments along the anterior–posterior hippocampal axis
to avoid signal mixture between the anterior and posterior
hippocampus.
For each subject, the mean time courses of aHPC and
pHPC were calculated and were correlated with the time
courses of the whole brain to construct bilateral aHPC and
pHPC functional connectivity maps. These maps were then
converted to z value maps using Fisher’s r-to-z transforma-
tion and smoothed with a Gaussian kernel with a full width
half maximum of 6mm.
Statistical analysis
We examined associations between maternal sensitivity
and the aHPC and pHPC functional connectivity maps in
children at both 4 and 6years of age using a mixed-effects
FLAME 1 model implemented in FSL. Maternal educa-
tion and ethnicity, age at MRI scan, postnatal maternal
depressive symptoms at 3months postpartum, and FD were
included as covariates. These factors were taken into account
because maternal education and ethnicity have been shown
Fig. 1 Illustration of the anterior and posterior hippocampal seed
regions. The whole hippocampus was divided into the anterior and
posterior section based on the location of uncal apex in the MNI
space (i.e., Y = − 21mm). To avoid contamination effects between the
aHPC and pHPC, a 2-mm coronal slice from each of the two adjacent
ends was removed. L left, aHPC anterior hippocampus, pHPC poste-
rior hippocampus
1890 Brain Structure and Function (2019) 224:1885–1895
1 3
to affect maternal mood (González etal. 2010), and age at
MRI and FD can influence the functional network (Van Dijk
etal. 2012).
In regression analysis, covariates were entered into the
first block of equations. In the second block, mean-centered
maternal sensitivity and sex were entered. The interaction
term, the product of mean-centered maternal sensitivity and
sex, was entered into the third block. When the interactive
effect was not significant, a reduced model, controlling for
the same covariates and sex, examined maternal sensitivity
in relation to the same outcome measures. Statistical results
were determined at a cluster level (z > 3.1, p < 0.001) and at a
family-wise error rate of 0.05 for the correction for multiple
comparisons (Eklund etal. 2016).
Results
Demographics
The 4- and 6-year-old samples did not differ in APGAR score
(t(135) = 0.515, p = 0.608), gestational age (t(135) = − 1.657,
p = 0.100), birth weight (t(135) = 0.199, p = 0.842), postna-
tal maternal depression (t(135) = 0.714, p = 0.477), mater-
nal sensitivity (t(135) = 0.482, p = 0.631), sex (χ2
(1) = 1.899,
p = 0.168), maternal ethnicity (χ2
(2) = 0.794, p = 0.672) and
maternal education (χ2
(4) = 4.058, p = 0.398). The 4- and
6-year-old samples, respectively, had 4 and 2 subjects whose
gestational age was less than 37weeks. Only 35 subjects
had maternal sensitivity as well as both 4-year and 6-year
rs-fMRI data. Due to this small sample at both time points,
we did not examine our results in a longitudinal manner.
Maternal sensitivity did not vary significantly as a func-
tion of postnatal maternal depressive symptoms (r = − 0.005,
p = 0.970), gestational age (r = 0.029, p = 0.823), sex
(t(59) = − 0.651, p = 0.517), and birth weight (r = − 0.033,
p = 0.802) in the 4-year-old sample. Similarly, maternal sen-
sitivity was not significantly associated with postnatal mater-
nal depressive symptoms (r = − 0.142, p = 0.223), gestational
age (r = 0.182, p = 0.115), sex (t(74) = − 0.901, p = 0.371), and
birth weight (r = 0.071, p = 0.545) in the 6-year-old sample.
Associations ofmaternal sensitivity withanterior
andposterior hippocampal functional networks
The interaction between maternal sensitivity and sex did not
predict the bilateral aHPC and pHPC functional networks at
4 or 6years of age. Next, we report results from the reduced
models.
Four year olds Maternal sensitivity during infancy was
positively associated with 4-year-old’s functional con-
nectivity between the right aHPC and the right precentral
gyrus (corrected p = 0.001), the left postcentral gyrus (cor-
rected p < 0.001) and the right postcentral gyrus (corrected
p < 0.001). Maternal sensitivity was negatively associated
with functional connectivity between the right aHPC and
the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC) (corrected
p = 0.016) (Fig.2). Table2 lists the anatomical coordinates
of these regions in the MNI space and the cluster size of
these findings. These results remained significant even
when the premature subjects (GA < 37weeks, n = 4) were
excluded (see Figure S2 in the Supplementary Material). In
sum, these findings indicated that maternal sensitivity pre-
dominantly predicted aHPC’s functional connectivity with
sensorimotor and top–down cognitive control networks in
4-year-old children.
Six year olds Maternal sensitivity during infancy was posi-
tively associated with theright aHPC functional connec-
tivity and aspects of the visual-processing network, includ-
ing the left calcarine (corrected p < 0.001), r ight calcarine
(corrected p < 0.001), right lingual (corrected p < 0.001),
and left cuneus cortex (corrected p < 0.001) in 6year olds
(Fig.3). The anatomical coordinates and cluster sizes of
these findings are listed in Table2. When the premature
children (GA < 37weeks, n = 2) were excluded, these results
remained significant (see Figure S3 in the Supplementary
Material). Together, these findings suggested that maternal
sensitivity predicted the aHPC’s functional connectivity
with the visual-processing network in 6-year-old children.
No significant findings were observed with regards to
the bilateral pHPC and left aHPC functional networks in
4- or 6-year-old children.
Fig. 2 Influences of maternal sensitivity on the right anterior hip-
pocampal functional network in 4-year-old sample. L left, R right,
dlPFC dorsolateral prefrontal cortex
1891Brain Structure and Function (2019) 224:1885–1895
1 3
Discussion
Here, we observed that maternal sensitivity, assessed at
6months postpartum, predicted theright aHPC functional
networks in children at both 4 and 6years of age. When
children were 4years of age, previously assessed levels
of maternal sensitivity were positively related to the right
aHPC’s functional connectivity with the sensorimotor net-
work and negatively to the right aHPC’s functional con-
nectivity with the top–down cognitive control network.
When children were 6years of age, previously assessed
levels of maternal sensitivity were positively linked to the
right aHPC’s functional connectivity with the visual-pro-
cessing network. Our findings suggested that maternal sen-
sitivity in infancy has a long-term impact on the anterior
hippocampal functional networks in preschool children.
The sensorimotor network is mainly composed of the
precentral and postcentral cortex (Berman etal. 2016). Its
developmental course starts early and peaks at 2–3years
of age (Casey etal. 2005). Around 4years of age, its con-
nections to the hippocampus are greater in the aHPC rather
than pHPC regions (Riggins etal. 2016). The sensorimotor
development in early life is critical and vulnerable to expo-
sure to psychological adversity, such as maternal anxiety
and depression and maternal care (Sale etal. 2009). In the
same GUSTO sample, we previously employed diffusion
tensor imaging (DTI) and reported that antenatal maternal
anxiety predicted variation in the microstructure of the
precentral and postcentral cortex as well as the medial
temporal lobe (Rifkin-Graboi etal. 2015b). Maternal anxi-
ety at 19weeks gestation was associated with gray matter
volume reductions in the medial temporal lobe and the
postcentral cortex in 6–9-year-old children (Buss etal.
2010). Moreover, a preliminary study with a small sam-
ple (n = 20) showed that maternal sensitivity influences
the hippocampal volume and functional organization in
6-month-old infants (Rifkin-Graboi etal. 2015a, b). Fur-
thermore, abnormalities in the sensorimotor functional
network have been identified in children with withdrawn
behavioral problems assessed using child behavior check-
list (CBCL) at early childhood (Wee etal. 2018). These
findings suggest that maternal psychological adversity and
behavior might manipulate the development of the senso-
rimotor systems.
In contrast to the enhanced coupling between the aHPC
and sensorimotor networks, we observed decoupling
between the aHPC and the cognitive control network as a
function of maternal sensitivity. Such aberrant aHPC-pre-
frontal functional connectivity has also been observed in
studies focused upon the association between early adver-
sity (e.g., childhood poverty and childhood maltreatment)
and neurodevelopment in school-age (7–12) children (Barch
etal. 2016; Birn etal. 2014). Our previous study with a
limited sample (n = 20) from the same GUSTO cohort sug-
gested that maternal sensitivity at6months postpartum was
associated with increased coupling between the hippocam-
pus and dlPFC in 6-month-old infants (Rifkin-Graboi etal.
2015a, b). Together, these findings suggest that directional
Table 2 Statistical associations
between maternal sensitivity
(MS) and right anterior
hippocampal functional
networks
Below lists anatomical structures, their coordinates in the MNI space, and cluster size
Positive and negative, respectively, indicate positive and negative correlations between maternal sensitivity
and the right anterior hippocampal functional network
MS maternal sensitivity, dlPFC dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, L left, R right
Sample Effects Anatomy Peak MNI coordinate Cluster
size (vox-
els)
X Y Z
4year olds MS (positive) R Precentral 20 − 28 54 39
L Postcentral − 36 − 34 58 171
R Postcentral 40 − 30 58 95
MS (negative) L dlPFC − 34 52 14 79
6year olds MS (positive) L Calcarine − 22 − 64 6 79
R Calcarine 12 − 68 12 77
R Lingual 20 − 56 2 41
L Cuneus − 8 − 72 20 13
Fig. 3 Influences of maternal sensitivity on the right anterior hip-
pocampal functional network in the 6-year-old sample. L left, R right
1892 Brain Structure and Function (2019) 224:1885–1895
1 3
effects of the early caregiving environment upon the aHPC-
prefrontal functional connectivity may be influenced by age
at assessment. Indeed, our additional analysis (see Figure
S4 in the Supplementary Material) showed that age does
modulate the relationship of maternal sensitivity and the
aHPC’s functional network. Similarly, past research showed
that the aHPC-prefrontal functional connectivity negatively
predicts episodic memory in 4year olds but positively pre-
dicts episodic memory in 6year olds (Riggins etal. 2016).
It is unclear why the relationship of the aHPC-prefrontal
functional connectivity with maternal care or child cognition
shifts from positive to negative association from younger
to older age in early life. Nevertheless, this phenomenon
is not unique to the hippocampus. In typical development,
the amygdala-prefrontal functional organization also shifts
from positive to negative connectivity from childhood to
adolescence (Gee etal. 2013; Gabard-Durnam etal. 2014). It
has been suggested that earlier amygdala development may
drive heavier bottom–up signaling early in life, which accel-
erates the prefrontal development (Gee etal. 2013; Gab-
ard-Durnam etal. 2014). As top–down signaling increas-
ingly emerges over time, the prefrontal cortex plays a role
in regulating signals from the amygdala (Gee etal. 2013;
Gabard-Durnam etal. 2014). This intriguing model may
be applied to the general development of regulatory con-
nections between the amygdala-hippocampal complex and
prefrontal cortex. In our study, this shift happened much
earlier, suggesting that early childhood demarcates as a criti-
cal and malleable period in the hippocampal-cortical forma-
tion, along with increased vulnerability to environmental
influences.
Finally, in the current study, higher levels of maternal
sensitivity were associated with increased functional con-
nectivity between the aHPC and the visual-processing net-
work in 6year olds. The visual-processing network identi-
fied in the present study consisted of the lingual, cuneus
and calcarine cortex. Prior studies have indicated that the
lingual cortex enhances activity during spatial and visual
working memory (Migo etal. 2015), associates with chronic
perceived stress (Veer etal. 2010), and is linked to resilience
to childhood maltreatment (van der Werff etal. 2013). The
calcarine cortex is thought to play a role in the perception
of visual cues for saccades (Lalli etal. 2006) and visual-
mental imagery (Klein etal. 2000), as well as positive affect
processing (Malhi etal. 2007; Park etal. 2010; Killgore and
Yurgelun-Todd 2007). In line with the findings in this study,
our previous study on 6-month infants (n = 20) showed the
positive associations of maternal sensitivity with the func-
tional connectivity between the hippocampus and the vis-
ual-processing network (Rifkin-Graboi etal. 2015a). These
findings suggest the long-term impact of maternal care on
the hippocampal and visual-processing network. However,
further investigation is needed to find out the relationships
among maternal care, hippocampal and visual-processing
functional organization, and child cognitive and behavioral
outcomes.
In our study, we showed that maternal sensitivity selec-
tively affected the anterior but not the posterior hippocampal
functional networks at both 4 and 6years of age. A substan-
tial body of literature supports the associations of maternal
psychological factors and care with children’s socioemo-
tional behavior (Leerkes etal. 2009; Palmer etal. 2018).
And a consensus from the literature suggests the aPHC is
involved in stress regulation, emotion, and affect (Fanselow
and Dong 2010). This evidence together with our findings
may potentially suggest that the aPHC functional organiza-
tion might be vulnerable to maternal care and play a crucial
role in early socioemotional development.
Our study highlighted the importance of the right but not
left aHPC functional organization in relation to maternal
sensitivity. Our previous study with a small sample (n = 35)
also showed the growth of the right hippocampal volume in
the first 6months of life in association with maternal anxiety
(Qiu etal. 2013). The right hippocampal volume was selec-
tively associated with chronic perceived stress (Gianaros
etal. 2007). It is unclear what are underlying mechanisms
for this specific laterality of the hippocampus sensitive to
stress and maternal psychological factors and care. One sug-
gested explanation was that an asymmetric concentration of
stress-related neurotransmitters, such as serotonin, may play
a role in manipulating right-lateralized hippocampal func-
tions (Bremner etal. 1995).
There were several limitations of this study. Head motion
of rs-fMRI is considered a major factor that influences the
rs-fMRI signal. Nevertheless, there is no gold standard to
characterize the potential effects of head motion on the rs-
fMRI signal. This study utilized a global signal regression
to remove micro-motion that cannot be characterized using
motion parameters (e.g., rotation and translation) (Power
etal. 2014). Even though there is a debate that a global
signal regression could induce spurious negative correla-
tion, we performed additional analysis and demonstrated
that the global signal regression at least did not influence
our findings (see Figure S5 in the Supplementary Material).
Moreover, the sample size of this study was relatively large
for a pediatric imaging study at 4 and 6year olds. Nev-
ertheless, the overlap between 4- and 6-year-old samples
was small, limiting our ability to capture longitudinal tra-
jectories. This research, then, calls for further studies of the
relation between maternal sensitivity and the developmental
trajectory of hippocampal functional networks.
In summary, our findings suggest a long-term impact of
maternal sensitivity during infancy upon anterior hippocam-
pal functional networks in early childhood. Our findings sug-
gest a potential role of maternal care in shaping early-life
child brain development and underscore the importance
1893Brain Structure and Function (2019) 224:1885–1895
1 3
of public health efforts to enhance maternal care during
infancy.
Funding This research is supported by the Singapore National
Research Foundation under its Translational and Clinical Research
(TCR) Flagship Programme and administered by the Singapore Min-
istry of Health’s National Medical Research Council (NMRC), Sin-
gapore- NMRC/TCR/004-NUS/2008; NMRC/TCR/012-NUHS/2014.
Additional funding is provided by the Singapore Institute for Clinical
Sciences, Agency for Science Technology and Research (A*STAR),
Singapore Ministry of Education (Academic research fund tier 1;
NUHSRO/2017/052/T1-SRP-Partnership/01), and NUS Institute of
Data Science, Singapore.
Compliance with ethical standards
Conflict of interest The authors declare that they have no conflict of
interest.
Ethnical approval All procedures performed in studies involving
human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of
the institutional and national research committee and with the 1964
Helsinki Declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical
standards.
Informed consent Informed consent was obtained from all individual
participants included in the study.
References
Ainsworth MD, Blehar MC, Waters E, Wall S (1978) Patterns of
attachment: assessed in the strange situation and at home. Erl-
baum, Hillsdale
Bagot RC, Zhang T-Y, Wen X, Nguyen TTT, Nguyen H-B, Diorio
J, Wong TP, Meaney MJ (2012) Variations in postnatal mater-
nal care and the epigenetic regulation of metabotropic glutamate
receptor 1 expression and hippocampal function in the rat. Proc
Natl Acad Sci USA 109(Supplement 2):17200–17207
Bannerman D, Rawlins J, McHugh S, Deacon R, Yee B, Bast T, Zhang
W-N, Pothuizen H, Feldon J (2004) Regional dissociations within
the hippocampus—memory and anxiety. Neurosci Biobehav Rev
28(3):273–283
Barch D, Pagliaccio D, Belden A, Harms MP, Gaffrey M, Sylvester
CM, Tillman R, Luby J (2016) Effect of hippocampal and amyg-
dala connectivity on the relationship between preschool poverty
and school-age depression. Am J Psychiatry 173(6):625–634
Bergink V, Kooistra L, Lambregtse-van den Berg MP, Wijnen H,
Bunevicius R, van Baar A, Pop V (2011) Validation of the Edin-
burgh Depression Scale during pregnancy. J Psychosom Res
70(4):385–389
Berman BD, Smucny J, Wylie KP, Shelton E, Kronberg E, Leehey M,
Tregellas JR (2016) Levodopa modulates small-world architecture
of functional brain networks in Parkinson’s disease. Movement
Disorders 31(11):1676–1684
Bernier A, Dégeilh F, Leblanc É, Daneault V, Bailey HN, Beauchamp
MH (2019) Mother–infant interaction and child brain morphol-
ogy: a multidimensional approach to maternal sensitivity. Infancy
24(2):120–138
Birn RM, Shackman AJ, Oler JA, Williams LE, McFarlin DR, Rogers
GM, Shelton SE, Alexander AL, Pine DS, Slattery MJ (2014)
Evolutionarily conserved prefrontal-amygdalar dysfunction in
early-life anxiety. Mol Psychiatry 19(8):915–922
Blair C, Granger D, Willoughby M, Kivlighan K (2006) Maternal
sensitivity is related to hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis
stress reactivity and regulation in response to emotion challenge
in 6-month-old infants. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1094(1):263–267
Bremner JD, Randall P, Scott TM, Bronen RA, Seibyl JP, Southwick
SM, Delaney RC, McCarthy G, Charney DS, Innis RB (1995)
MRI-based measurement of hippocampal volume in patients
with combat-related posttraumatic stress disorder. Am J Psy-
chiatry 152(7):973–981
Buss C, Davis EP, Shahbaba B, Pruessner JC, Head K, Sandman
CA (2012) Maternal cortisol over the course of pregnancy and
subsequent child amygdala and hippocampus volumes and affec-
tive problems. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 109(20):E1312–E1319
Cabeza R, St Jacques P (2007) Functional neuroimaging of autobio-
graphical memory. Trends Cogn Sci 11(5):219–227
Casey B, Tottenham N, Liston C, Durston S (2005) Imaging the
developing brain: what have we learned about cognitive devel-
opment? Trends Cogn Sci 9(3):104–110
Champagne DL, Bagot RC, van Hasselt F, Ramakers G, Meaney
MJ, De Kloet ER, Joëls M, Krugers H (2008) Maternal care
and hippocampal plasticity: evidence for experience-dependent
structural plasticity, altered synaptic functioning, and differen-
tial responsiveness to glucocorticoids and stress. J Neurosci
28(23):6037–6045
Desikan RS, Ségonne F, Fischl B, Quinn BT, Dickerson BC, Blacker
D, Buckner RL, Dale AM, Maguire RP, Hyman BT (2006) An
automated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral
cortex on MRI scans into gyral based regions of interest. Neu-
roimage 31(3):968–980
Drury SS (2012) Maternal sensitivity and attachment: Softening the
impact of early adversity. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry
51(7):670–672
Du J, Younes L, Qiu A (2011) Whole brain diffeomorphic metric
mapping via integration of sulcal and gyral curves, cortical
surfaces, and images. Neuroimage 56(1):162–173. https ://doi.
org/10.1016/j.neuro image .2011.01.067
Eklund A, Nichols TE, Knutsson H (2016) Cluster failure: why fMRI
inferences for spatial extent have inflated false-positive rates.
Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 113(28):7900–7905
Fanselow MS, Dong H-W (2010) Are the dorsal and ventral hip-
pocampus functionally distinct structures? Neuron 65(1):7–19
Faure N, Habersaat S, Harari MM, Müller-Nix C, Borghini A, Anser-
met F, Tolsa J-F, Urben S (2017) Maternal sensitivity: a resil-
ience factor against internalizing symptoms in early adolescents
born very preterm? J Abnorm Child Psychol 45(4):671–680
Frick MA, Forslund T, Fransson M, Johansson M, Bohlin G, Brocki
KC (2018) The role of sustained attention, maternal sensitiv-
ity, and infant temperament in the development of early self-
regulation. Br J Psychol 109(2):277–298
Gabard-Durnam LJ, Flannery J, Goff B, Gee DG, Humphreys
KL, Telzer E, Hare T, Tottenham N (2014) The development
of human amygdala functional connectivity at rest from 4 to
23years: a cross-sectional study. Neuroimage 95:193–207. https
://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuro image .2014.03.038
Gee DG, Gabard-Durnam LJ, Flannery J, Goff B, Humphreys KL,
Telzer EH, Hare TA, Bookheimer SY, Tottenham N (2013)
Early developmental emergence of human amygdala-prefron-
tal connectivity after maternal deprivation. Proc Natl Acad Sci
USA 110(39):15638–15643. https ://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.13078
93110
Gianaros PJ, Jennings JR, Sheu LK, Greer PJ, Kuller LH, Matthews
KA (2007) Prospective reports of chronic life stress predict
decreased grey matter volume in the hippocampus. Neuroimage
35(2):795–803
1894 Brain Structure and Function (2019) 224:1885–1895
1 3
González HM, Tarraf W, Whitfield KE, Vega WA (2010) The epide-
miology of major depression and ethnicity in the United States. J
Psychiatr Res 44(15):1043–1051
Hassabis D, Kumaran D, Maguire EA (2007) Using imagination to
understand the neural basis of episodic memory. J Neurosci
27(52):14365–14374
Herman JP, Ostrander MM, Mueller NK, Figueiredo H (2005) Limbic
system mechanisms of stress regulation: hypothalamo-pituitary-
adrenocortical axis. Progr Neuro-Psychopharmacol Biol Psychia-
try 29(8):1201–1213
Ispa JM, Su-Russell C, Palermo F, Carlo G (2017) The interplay of
maternal sensitivity and toddler engagement of mother in predict-
ing self-regulation. Dev Psychol 53(3):425–435
Killgore WD, Yurgelun-Todd DA (2007) Positive affect modulates
activity in the visual cortex to images of high calorie foods. Int J
Neurosci 117(5):643–653
Klein I, Paradis A-L, Poline J-B, Kosslyn SM, Le Bihan D (2000)
Transient activity in the human calcarine cortex during visual-
mental imagery: an event-related fMRI study. J Cogn Neurosci
12(Supplement 2):15–23
Lalli S, Hussain Z, Ayub A, Cracco R, Bodis-Wollner I, Amassian V
(2006) Role of the calcarine cortex (V1) in perception of visual
cues for saccades. Clin Neurophysiol 117(9):2030–2038
Leerkes EM, Blankson AN, O’Brien M (2009) Differential effects of
maternal sensitivity to infant distress and nondistress on social-
emotional functioning. Child Dev 80(3):762–775
Liu D, Diorio J, Tannenbaum B, Caldji C, Francis D, Freedman
A, Sharma S, Pearson D, Plotsky PM, Meaney MJ (1997)
Maternal care, hippocampal glucocorticoid receptors, and
hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal responses to stress. Science
277(5332):1659–1662
Luby J, Belden A, Harms MP, Tillman R, Barch DM (2016a) Preschool
is a sensitive period for the influence of maternal support on the
trajectory of hippocampal development. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA
113(20):5742–5747. https ://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.16014 43113
Luby JL, Belden A, Harms MP, Tillman R, Barch DM (2016b) Pre-
school is a sensitive period for the influence of maternal support
on the trajectory of hippocampal development. Proc Natl Acad
Sci USA 113(20):5742–5747
Mahar I, Bambico FR, Mechawar N, Nobrega JN (2014) Stress, sero-
tonin, and hippocampal neurogenesis in relation to depression
and antidepressant effects. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 38:173–192
Maheu FS, Dozier M, Guyer AE, Mandell D, Peloso E, Poeth K, Jen-
ness J, Lau JY, Ackerman JP, Pine DS (2010) A preliminary study
of medial temporal lobe function in youths with a history of car-
egiver deprivation and emotional neglect. Cogn Affect Behav
Neurosci 10(1):34–49
Malhi GS, Lagopoulos J, Owen AM, Ivanovski B, Shnier R, Sachdev P
(2007) Reduced activation to implicit affect induction in euthymic
bipolar patients: an fMRI study. J Affect Disord 97(1–3):109–122
Migo E, Mitterschiffthaler M, O’Daly O, Dawson G, Dourish C, Craig
K, Simmons A, Wilcock G, McCulloch E, Jackson S (2015) Alter-
ations in working memory networks in amnestic mild cognitive
impairment. Aging Neuropsychol Cogn 22(1):106–127
Mori S, Oishi K, Jiang H, Jiang L, Li X, Akhter K, Hua K, Faria
AV, Mahmood A, Woods R, Toga AW, Pike GB, Neto PR, Evans
A, Zhang J, Huang H, Miller MI, van Zijl P, Mazziotta J (2008)
Stereotaxic white matter atlas based on diffusion tensor imaging
in an ICBM template. Neuroimage 40(2):570–582. https ://doi.
org/10.1016/j.neuro image .2007.12.035
Nadel L, Hupbach A, Gomez R, Newman-Smith K (2012) Memory
formation, consolidation and transformation. Neurosci Biobehav
Rev 36(7):1640–1645
Nguyen H-B, Bagot RC, Diorio J, Wong TP, Meaney MJ (2015) Mater-
nal care differentially affects neuronal excitability and synaptic
plasticity in the dorsal and ventral hippocampus. Neuropsychop-
harmacology 40(7):1590–1599
Paavola L, Kemppinen K, Kumpulainen K, Moilanen I, Ebeling H
(2006) Maternal sensitivity, infant co-operation and early linguis-
tic development: Some predictive relations. Eur J Dev Psychol
3(1):13–30
Palmer FB, Graff JC, Jones TL, Murphy LE, Keisling BL, Whitaker
TM, Wang L, Tylavsky FA (2018) Socio-demographic, maternal,
and child indicators of socioemotional problems in 2-year-old
children: a cohort study. Medicine (Baltimore) 97(28):e11468.
https ://doi.org/10.1097/md.00000 00000 01146 8
Park J-Y, Gu B-M, Kang D-H, Shin Y-W, Choi C-H, Lee J-M, Kwon
JS (2010) Integration of cross-modal emotional information in the
human brain: an fMRI study. Cortex 46(2):161–169
Poppenk J, Evensmoen HR, Moscovitch M, Nadel L (2013) Long-
axis specialization of the human hippocampus. Trends Cogn Sci
17(5):230–240
Power JD, Barnes KA, Snyder AZ, Schlaggar BL, Petersen SE
(2012) Spurious but systematic correlations in functional con-
nectivity MRI networks arise from subject motion. NeuroImage
59(3):2142–2154
Power JD, Mitra A, Laumann TO, Snyder AZ, Schlaggar BL, Petersen
SE (2014) Methods to detect, characterize, and remove motion
artifact in resting state fMRI. NeuroImage 84:320–341
Qiu A, Rifkin-Graboi A, Chen H, Chong Y, Kwek K, Gluckman
P, Fortier M, Meaney M (2013) Maternal anxiety and infants
hippocampal development: timing matters. Transl Psychiatry
3(9):e306
Rao H, Betancourt L, Giannetta JM, Brodsky NL, Korczykowski M,
Avants BB, Gee JC, Wang J, Hurt H, Detre JA (2010) Early paren-
tal care is important for hippocampal maturation: evidence from
brain morphology in humans. Neuroimage 49(1):1144–1150
Rifkin-Graboi A, Kong L, Sim L, Sanmugam S, Broekman B, Chen
H, Wong E, Kwek K, Saw S, Chong Y (2015a) Maternal sensitiv-
ity, infant limbic structure volume and functional connectivity: a
preliminary study. Transl Psychiatry 5(10):e668
Rifkin-Graboi A, Meaney MJ, Chen H, Bai J, Hameed WBR, Tint MT,
Broekman BF, Chong Y-S, Gluckman PD, Fortier MV (2015b)
Antenatal maternal anxiety predicts variations in neural structures
implicated in anxiety disorders in newborns. J Am Acad Child
Adolesc Psychiatry 54(4):313–321
Riggins T, Geng F, Blankenship SL, Redcay E (2016) Hippocampal
functional connectivity and episodic memory in early childhood.
Dev Cogn Neurosci 19:58–69
Sale A, Berardi N, Maffei L (2009) Enrich the environment to empower
the brain. Trends Neurosci 32(4):233–239
Sapolsky RM, Krey LC, McEWEN BS (1985) Prolonged glucocorti-
coid exposure reduces hippocampal neuron number: implications
for aging. J Neurosci 5(5):1222–1227
Satpute AB, Mumford JA, Naliboff BD, Poldrack RA (2012) Human
anterior and posterior hippocampus respond distinctly to state and
trait anxiety. Emotion 12(1):58–68
Smith SM, Jenkinson M, Woolrich MW, Beckmann CF, Behrens TE,
Johansen-Berg H, Bannister PR, De Luca M, Drobnjak I, Flit-
ney DE (2004) Advances in functional and structural MR image
analysis and implementation as FSL. Neuroimage 23:S208–S219
Soe NN, Wen DJ, Poh JS, Li Y, Broekman BF, Chen H, Chong YS,
Kwek K, Saw SM, Gluckman PD, Meaney MJ, Rifkin-Graboi A,
Qiu A (2016) Pre- and Post-Natal Maternal Depressive Symp-
toms in Relation with Infant Frontal Function, Connectivity, and
Behaviors. PLoS One 11(4):e0152991. https ://doi.org/10.1371/
journ al.pone.01529 91
Soh SE, Lee SSM, Hoon SW, Tan MY, Goh A, Lee BW, Shek LP-C,
Teoh OH, Kwek K, Saw SM, Godfrey K, Chong YS, Gluckman
P, van Bever HPS (2012) The methodology of the GUSTO cohort
1895Brain Structure and Function (2019) 224:1885–1895
1 3
study: a novel approach in studying pediatric allergy. Asia Pac
Allergy 2(2):144–148
Tan M, Qiu A (2016) Large deformation multiresolution diffeomorphic
metric mapping for multiresolution cortical surfaces: a coarse-to-
fine approach. IEEE Trans Image Process 25(9):4061–4074
Tarabulsy GM, Provost MA, Bordeleau S, Trudel-Fitzgerald C, Moran
G, Pederson DR, Trabelsi M, Lemelin JP, Pierce T (2009) Valida-
tion of a short version of the maternal behavior Q-set applied to
a brief video record of mother-infant interaction. Infant Behav
Dev 32(1):132–136. https ://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbe h.2008.09.006
Treyvaud K, Doyle LW, Lee KJ, Ure A, Inder TE, Hunt RW, Ander-
son PJ (2016) Parenting behavior at 2 years predicts school-age
performance at 7 years in very preterm children. J Child Psychol
Psyc 57(7):814–821
van der Werff SJ, Pannekoek JN, Veer IM, van Tol M-J, Aleman A,
Veltman DJ, Zitman FG, Rombouts SA, Elzinga BM, van der
Wee NJ (2013) Resilience to childhood maltreatment is associ-
ated with increased resting-state functional connectivity of the
salience network with the lingual gyrus. Child Abuse Neglect
37(11):1021–1029
Van Dijk KR, Sabuncu MR, Buckner RL (2012) The influence of head
motion on intrinsic functional connectivity MRI. NeuroImage
59(1):431–438
Veer IM, Beckmann C, Van Tol M-J, Ferrarini L, Milles J, Veltman
D, Aleman A, Van Buchem MA, Van Der Wee NJ, Rombouts
SA (2010) Whole brain resting-state analysis reveals decreased
functional connectivity in major depression. Front Syst Neurosci
4:1–10
Weaver IC, Meaney MJ, Szyf M (2006) Maternal care effects on the
hippocampal transcriptome and anxiety-mediated behaviors in the
offspring that are reversible in adulthood. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA
103(9):3480–3485
Wee C-Y, Poh JS, Wang Q, Broekman BF, Chong Y-S, Kwek K, Shek
LP, Saw S-M, Gluckman PD, Fortier MV (2018) Behavioral het-
erogeneity in relation with brain functional networks in young
children. Cereb Cortex 28(9):3322–3331
Wen DJ, Poh JS, Ni SN, Chong YS, Chen H, Kwek K, Shek LP,
Gluckman PD, Fortier MV, Meaney MJ, Qiu A (2017a) Influ-
ences of prenatal and postnatal maternal depression on amygdala
volume and microstructure in young children. Transl Psychiatry
7(4):e1103. https ://doi.org/10.1038/tp.2017.74
Wen DJ, Soe NN, Sim LW, Sanmugam S, Kwek K, Chong YS, Gluck-
man PD, Meaney MJ, Rifkin-Graboi A, Qiu A (2017b) Infant
frontal EEG asymmetry in relation with postnatal maternal
depression and parenting behavior. Transl Psychiatry 7(3):e1057.
https ://doi.org/10.1038/tp.2017.28
Whittle S, Simmons JG, Dennison M, Vijayakumar N, Schwartz O, Yap
MB, Sheeber L, Allen NB (2014) Positive parenting predicts the
development of adolescent brain structure: a longitudinal study.
Dev Cogn Neurosci 8:7–17
Zhang Y, Zhang J, Hsu J, Oishi K, Faria AV, Albert M, Miller MI, Mori
S (2014) Evaluation of group-specific, whole-brain atlas genera-
tion using Volume-based Template Estimation (VTE): application
to normal and Alzheimer’s populations. Neuroimage 84:406–419.
https ://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuro image .2013.09.011
Zhong J, Phua DY, Qiu A (2010) Quantitative evaluation of LDDMM,
FreeSurfer, and CARET for cortical surface mapping. Neuroim-
age 52(1):131–141
Publisher’s Note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to
jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
... Although more negative caregiving environments and more a negative caregiver emotional state may represent a significant risk factor for differences in hippocampal development, studies report that high-quality caregiving may be particularly beneficial or protective (Luby et al., 2013(Luby et al., , 2016(Luby et al., , 2019Wang et al., 2014Wang et al., , 2019. Overall, better caregiving quality is associated with larger left hippocampi and smaller right hippocampi in infancy (Qiu et al., 2013), greater anterior functional connectivity in early childhood (Wang et al., 2019), faster growth in volume at preschool age (Luby et al., 2016), and larger volumes and functional networks during school age (Luby et al., 2013(Luby et al., , 2016(Luby et al., , 2019Wang et al., 2014). ...
... Although more negative caregiving environments and more a negative caregiver emotional state may represent a significant risk factor for differences in hippocampal development, studies report that high-quality caregiving may be particularly beneficial or protective (Luby et al., 2013(Luby et al., , 2016(Luby et al., , 2019Wang et al., 2014Wang et al., , 2019. Overall, better caregiving quality is associated with larger left hippocampi and smaller right hippocampi in infancy (Qiu et al., 2013), greater anterior functional connectivity in early childhood (Wang et al., 2019), faster growth in volume at preschool age (Luby et al., 2016), and larger volumes and functional networks during school age (Luby et al., 2013(Luby et al., , 2016(Luby et al., , 2019Wang et al., 2014). Findings are mixed in adolescence and young adulthood, as histories of early life adversity and negative caregiving have been associated with both smaller (Bremner et al., 1997;Stein et al., 1997;Vythilingam et al., 2002;Buss et al., 2007;Rao et al., 2010) and larger (Rao et al., 2010) hippocampi during adolescence (Belsky and de Haan, 2011). ...
Article
Full-text available
Early adversities, including prenatal drug exposure (PDE) and a negative postnatal emotional caregiving environment, impact children’s long-term development. The protracted developmental course of memory and its underlying neural systems offer a valuable framework for understanding the longitudinal associations of pre- and postnatal factors on children with PDE. This study longitudinally examines memory and hippocampal development in 69 parent–child dyads to investigate how the early caregiving emotional environment affects children with PDE’s neural and cognitive systems. Measures of physical health, drug exposure, caregiver stress, depression, and distress were collected between 0 and 24 months At age 14 years, adolescents completed multiple measures of episodic memory, and at ages 14 and 18 years, adolescents underwent magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) scans. Latent constructs of episodic memory and the caregiving environment were created using Confirmatory Factor Analysis. Multiple regressions revealed a negative emotional caregiving environment during infancy was associated with poor memory performance and smaller left hippocampal volumes at 14 years. Better memory performance at 14 years predicted larger right hippocampal volume at 18 years. At 18 years, the association between the emotional caregiving environment and hippocampal volume was moderated by sex, such that a negative emotional caregiving environment was associated with larger left hippocampal volumes in males but not females. Findings suggest that the postnatal caregiving environment may modulate the effects of PDE across development, influencing neurocognitive development.
... The number of conversational turns in a child's home environment has also been shown to be associated with greater activation in the left inferior frontal gyrus, and activation in this region was in turn associated with better verbal skills in four-to six-year-old children (Romeo et al., 2018). In addition to stimulation, parental sensitivity has also been shown to be associated with differences in both brain structure and function during early childhood, including total brain volume, gray matter volume, amygdala volume, and functioning of hippocampal networks (Bernier et al., 2019;Kok et al., 2015;Wang et al., 2019). Relatedly, another study using fMRI (in four-to 11-year-olds) found that negative parenting behaviors such as the use of a harsh tone and physical control were associated with lower amygdala activation during children's experience of positive emotional events (Park et al., 2022). ...
Article
Full-text available
The overarching goal of this paper is to examine the efficacy of early intervention when viewed through the lens of developmental neuroscience. We begin by briefly summarizing neural development from conception through the first few postnatal years. We emphasize the role of experience during the postnatal period, and consistent with decades of research on critical periods, we argue that experience can represent both a period of opportunity and a period of vulnerability. Because plasticity is at the heart of early intervention, we next turn our attention to the efficacy of early intervention drawing from two distinct literatures: early intervention services for children growing up in disadvantaged environments, and children at elevated likelihood of developing a neurodevelopmental delay or disorder. In the case of the former, we single out interventions that target caregiving and in the case of the latter, we highlight recent work on autism. A consistent theme throughout our review is a discussion of how early intervention is embedded in the developing brain. We conclude our article by discussing the implications our review has for policy, and we then offer recommendations for future research.
... exposure to warm and responsive parenting behavior, secure parent-child attachment) may influence child brain development. Specifically, normative variation in different indicators of the quality of early caregiving relationships predicts differences in child grey matter volume (Bernier et al., 2019;Kok et al., 2015;Leblanc et al., 2017;Lee et al., 2019;Luby et al., 2012Luby et al., , 2016Rifkin-Graboi et al., 2015;Sethna et al., 2017Sethna et al., , 2019, thickness (Kok et al., 2015;Leblanc et al., 2022), activity (Bernier et al., 2016;Biro et al., 2021;Hane et al., 2010), and functional connectivity (Dégeilh et al., 2018;Hanford et al., 2018;Perone & Gartstein, 2019;Wang et al., 2019;Rifkin-Graboi et al., 2015;Thijssen et al., 2017), as well as grey matter development in childhood and adolescence (Luby et al., 2016;Whittle et al., 2014). In spite of these increasingly documented links between normative variation in caregiving indices and child grey matter structure and functioning, including with the same cohort as in the current study (Bernier et al., 2019;Dégeilh et al., 2018;Leblanc et al., 2017Leblanc et al., , 2022, the associations between caregiving and child white matter have seldom been investigated, particularly in the general population. ...
Article
Early childhood experiences are considered to influence the strength and effectiveness of neural connections and thus the development of brain connectivity. As one of the most pervasive and potent early relational experiences, parent-child attachment is a prime candidate to account for experience-driven differences in brain development. Yet, knowledge of the effects of parent-child attachment on brain structure in typically developing children is scarce and largely limited to grey matter, whereas caregiving influences on white matter (i.e. neural connections) have seldom been explored. This study examined whether normative variation in mother-child attachment security predicts white matter microstructure in late childhood and explored associations with cognitive-inhibition. Mother-child attachment security was assessed using home observations when children (N = 32, 20 girls) were 15 and 26 months old. White matter microstructure was assessed using diffusion magnetic resonance imaging when children were 10 years old. Child cognitive-inhibition was tested when children were 11 years old. Results revealed a negative association between mother-toddler attachment security and child white matter microstructure organization, which in turn related to better child cognitive-inhibition. While preliminary given the sample size, these findings add to the growing literature that suggests that rich and positive experiences are likely to decelerate brain development.
... In addition, in a small (n = 18) subset of children we found an association between hippocampal subregions and the difference in performance for angry versus happy relational memory. As such, the current work adds to an increasing number of papers linking early life environmental exposures to memory biases (Rifkin-Graboi et al., 2021) as well as hippocampal development (Rao et al., 2010;Luby et al., 2012Luby et al., , 2013Luby et al., , 2016Rifkin-Graboi et al., 2015;Bernier et al., 2019;Lee et al., 2019;Wang et al., 2019), potentially important to the development of children's adaptive functioning and psychological health. ...
Article
Full-text available
Introduction: Links between maternal sensitivity, hippocampal development, and memory abilities suggests early life insensitive care may shape structures and schemas influencing future decisions and stress management, biasing children to negative information. While it is possible that this pattern of neurodevelopment may have adaptive consequences, for example, preventing children from encountering untoward experience with future adversity, it may also leave some children at risk for the development of internalizing problems. Methods: Here, in a Two Wave Study, we examine whether insensitive care predicts sub sequentially assessed memory biases for threatening (but not happy) stimuli in preschoolers (n = 49), and if such relations cut across different forms of relational memory, i.e., memory for relations between two "items," between an "item" and its spatial location, and an "item" and its temporal sequence. In a subset (n = 18) we also examine links between caregiving, memory, and hippocampal subregion volume. Results: Results indicate no main or interactive influence of gender on relational memory. However, insensitive caregiving predicted the difference between Angry and Happy memory during the Item-Space condition (B = 2.451, se = 0.969, p = 0.014, 95% CI (0.572, 4.340)], as well as memory for Angry (but not Happy) items [B = -2.203, se = 0.551, p < 0.001, 95% CI (-3.264,-1.094)]. Memory for the difference between Angry and Happy stimuli in the Space condition associated with larger right hippocampal body volumes (Rho = 0.639, p = 0.004). No relations were observed with internalizing problems. Discussion: Results are discussed with reference to developmental stage and in consideration of whether negative biases may serve as an intermediate factor linking early life insensitive care and later socioemotional problems including an increased incidence of internalizing disorders.
... Los hallazgos de las investigaciones en cuanto a la conexión entre la sensibilidad materna y la conexión del hipocampo y la amígdala han demostrado que la alta sensibilidad materna se asoció con mayor conectividad funcional entre el hipocampo y las regiones involucradas en la regulación emocional, comunicación y cognición (Wang et al. 2019;D. Ilyka et al.). ...
Article
Full-text available
OBJETIVO: identificar las consecuencias neuropsicológicas emocionales y conductuales por el uso de dispositivos digitales METODO: construcción teórica a partir de revisión bibliográfica RESULTADOS: la familia en relación al apego se encuentran en constante distracción en los dispositivos móviles tanto padres como hijos, esta conducta podría estar relacionada con daños a largo plazo en el cerebro en formación de los infantes al igual que en el cerebro adolescente, incrementando los sentimientos de abandono y soledad debido a la carencia de las expresiones de afecto que se necesitan para establecer un apego saludable. CONCLUSIÓN: La calidad de la interacción entre padres e hijos se ve afectada si estos se encuentran distraídos en los dispositivos electrónicos aumentando esta conducta los índices de uso inadecuado de los teléfonos inteligentes, internet, adicción a los dispositivos, ansiedad y depresión.
... Another review by Ilyka et al. (2021) reviewed studies investigating the relationship between parent-infant behaviors and measures of the child's brain structure and function; these studies showed wide variation in the neuroimaging data, while interaction data was more consistent, and maternal sensitivity was the most investigated. Previously, maternal sensitivity had been shown to be associated, for example, with hippocampal distal functional connectivity (Wang et al., 2019), hippocampal volumes bilaterally (Rifkin-Graboi et al., 2015), subcortical gray matter volume (Sethna et al., 2017), and total brain volumes (Kok et al., 2015). Finally, to the best of our knowledge, no one has examined the association between maternal sensitivity and brain local functional connectivity. ...
Article
Full-text available
The quality of mother–child interaction, especially maternal sensitivity in caregiving behavior, plays an important role in a child’s later socioemotional development. Numerous studies have indicated associations between poor mother–child interaction and offspring brain structure and function, but more knowledge on how variation in the characteristics of early caregiving is associated with children’s brain structure and function is needed. We investigated whether maternal sensitivity at 8 or 30 months is associated with functional connectivity in a child’s brain at 5 years of age based on the FinnBrain Birth Cohort Study (17 and 39 mother–child dyads at 8 and 30 months, respectively, with an overlap of 13 dyads). Maternal sensitivity was assessed during a free play interaction using the Emotional Availability Scales at 8 and 30 months of the children’s age. Task-free functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) was acquired at the age of 5 years in 7-min scans while watching the Inscapes movie. Regional homogeneity (ReHo) maps were created from the fMRI data, and multiple regression analysis was performed to assess the relation between maternal sensitivity and ReHo. Maternal sensitivity at the age of 8 months was positively associated with children’s ReHo values within the medial prefrontal cortex. Distal connectivity of this region showed no significant association with maternal sensitivity in a seed-based connectivity analysis. No associations were found between maternal sensitivity during toddlerhood and brain functional connectivity. Together, these results suggest that maternal sensitivity, especially in infancy, may influence offspring brain functional connectivity. However, studies with larger sample sizes are warranted.
Article
Parenting is a critical influence on the development of children across the globe. This handbook brings together scholars with expertise on parenting science and interventions for a comprehensive review of current research. It begins with foundational theories and research topics, followed by sections on parenting children at different ages, factors that affect parenting such as parental mental health or socioeconomic status, and parenting children with different characteristics such as depressed and anxious children or youth who identify as LGBTQ. It concludes with a section on policy implications, as well as prevention and intervention programs that target parenting as a mechanism of change. Global perspectives and the cultural diversity of families are highlighted throughout. Offering in-depth analysis of key topics such as risky adolescent behavior, immigration policy, father engagement, family involvement in education, and balancing childcare and work, this is a vital resource for understanding the most effective policies to support parents in raising healthy children.
Article
Human infants are born needing their caregivers’ support to accomplish challenges related to both security and exploration. Accordingly, the quality of care infants receive influences their ability to appraise the degree of threat inherent to any challenge, signal needs for assistance, and regulate their responses. In this manner, parenting affects whether young children manage challenges with behavior or physiological responses. The extent to which stress physiology is repeatedly invoked in response to challenges, alongside variation in neural growth accompanying children’s exploratory behavior, in turn affects neurodevelopment and ultimately functioning with age. We discuss the processes through which this occurs, the potential impact on attachment schemas, and implications for intervention programs designed at improving parenting and well-being.
Article
Full-text available
Throughout infancy and early childhood, stable and secure relationships with caregivers are needed to promote optimal socioemotional (SE) and cognitive development. The objective is to examine socio-demographic, maternal, and child indicators of SE problems in 2-year-olds living in an urban-suburban community in the southern United States. Mother–infant pairs enrolled in a prospective pregnancy cohort study. Shelby County (Memphis), Tennessee. One thousand five hundred three women were recruited during their second trimester and followed with their children through the child's age of 2 years. Child SE development was measured by the Brief Infant-Toddler Social Emotional Assessment at 2 years of age. Mothers reported their own behavioral and mental health, temperament, parenting stress, and potential for child abuse during gestation and/or when their child was 1 year of age. Examiners measured maternal IQ during data collection at the child's age of 1 year. Child communication, cognitive development, and risk for autism spectrum disorder were assessed at 1 and 2 years of age. Multivariable regression models were developed to predict mother-reported SE problems. In bivariate analyses, multiple maternal behavioral and mental health indicators and child cognitive skills were associated with reported child SE problems at 2 years of age. Regression analyses, controlling for socio-demographic, maternal, and child variables, showed the following factors were independently associated with mother-reported child SE problems: maternal education of high school or less, lower maternal IQ, higher maternal cyclothymic temperament score, greater parenting stress, greater maternal psychological distress, lower child expressive communication score, and child risk for autism spectrum disorder. Socio-demographic variables accounted for the variance often attributed to race. Since mothers in the study were medically low-risk, generalizing these findings to medically high-risk mothers is unwarranted. In addition, these SE outcomes in 2-year-old children do not reflect the trajectory of SE development throughout early childhood. Attention to independent indicators of future SE problems in children may help identify individual children and families needing intervention and target public prevention/treatment programs in communities.
Article
Full-text available
The hippocampus of the rat loses neurons with age, a loss which may eventuate in some of the functional impairments typical of senescence. Cumulative exposure to corticosterone (CORT) over the lifespan may be a cause of this neuronal loss, as it is prevented by adrenalectomy at mid- age. In this study, we demonstrate that prolonged exposure to CORT accelerates the process of cell loss. Rats were injected daily with sufficient CORT to produce prolonged elevations of circulating titers within the high physiological range. Animals treated for 3 months (chronic subjects) resembled aged rats in a number of ways. First, both groups had extensive and persistent depletions of CORT receptors in the hippocampus; in the case of chronic rats, no recovery of receptor concentrations occurred 4 months after the end of steroid treatment. Second, autoradiographic analysis revealed that the receptor depletion was due, in part, to a loss of CORT-concentrating cells, especially in the CA3 cell field. Remaining cells bound significantly less [3H]corticosterone than did those of control rats. Finally, analysis of size distributions of hippocampal cell bodies indicated that chronic subjects lost neurons of the same size as those lost in the aged hippocampus. Furthermore, chronic subjects also had increased numbers of small, darkly staining cells of CA3; these corresponded in size to the dark glia whose numbers increase in the aged hippocampus, and which are thought to infiltrate in response to neuronal damage or destruction. Thus, this study supports the hypothesis that cumulative exposure to CORT over the lifespan may contribute to age-related loss of neurons in the hippocampus, and that prolonged stress or exposure to CORT accelerates this process.
Article
Full-text available
Maternal depressive symptoms influence neurodevelopment in the offspring. Such effects may appear to be gender-dependent. The present study examined contributions of prenatal and postnatal maternal depressive symptoms to the volume and microstructure of the amygdala in 4.5-year-old boys and girls. Prenatal maternal depressive symptoms were measured using the Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale (EPDS) at 26 weeks of gestation. Postnatal maternal depression was assessed at 3 months using the EPDS and at 1, 2, 3 and 4.5 years using the Beck’s Depression Inventory-II. Structural magnetic resonance imaging and diffusion tensor imaging were performed with 4.5-year-old children to extract the volume and fractional anisotropy (FA) values of the amygdala. Our results showed that greater prenatal maternal depressive symptoms were associated with larger right amygdala volume in girls, but not in boys. Increased postnatal maternal depressive symptoms were associated with higher right amygdala FA in the overall sample and girls, but not in boys. These results support the role of variation in right amygdala structure in transmission of maternal depression to the offspring, particularly to girls. The differential effects of prenatal and postnatal maternal depressive symptoms on the volume and FA of the right amygdala suggest the importance of the timing of exposure to maternal depressive symptoms in brain development of girls. This further underscores the need for intervention targeting both prenatal and postnatal maternal depression to girls in preventing adverse child outcomes.
Article
Full-text available
Right frontal electroencephalogram (EEG) asymmetry associates with negative affect and depressed mood, which, among children, are predicted by maternal depression and poor parenting. This study examined associations of maternal depression and maternal sensitivity with infant frontal EEG asymmetry based on 111 mother-6-month-infant dyads. There were no significant effects of postnatal maternal depression or maternal sensitivity, or their interaction, on infant EEG frontal asymmetry. However, in a subsample for which the infant spent at least 50% of his/her day time hours with his/her mother, both lower maternal sensitivity and higher maternal depression predicted greater relative right frontal EEG asymmetry. Our study further showed that greater relative right frontal EEG asymmetry of 6-month-old infants predicted their greater negative emotionality at 12 months of age. Our study suggested that among infants with sufficient postnatal maternal exposure, both maternal sensitivity and mental health are important influences on early brain development.
Article
Full-text available
Using data from the Early Head Start Research and Evaluation Project, a cross-lag mediation model was tested to examine longitudinal relations among low-income mothers’ sensitivity; toddlers’ engagement of their mothers; and toddler’s self-regulation at ages 1, 2, and 3 years (N = 2,958). Age 1 maternal sensitivity predicted self-regulation at ages 2 and 3 years, and age 2 engagement of mother mediated the relation between age 1 maternal sensitivity and age 3 self-regulation. Lagged relations from toddler self-regulation at ages 1 and 2 years to later maternal sensitivity were not significant, suggesting stronger influence from mother to toddler than vice versa. Model fit was similar regardless of child gender and depth of family poverty.
Article
Full-text available
Background: PD is associated with disrupted connectivity to a large number of distributed brain regions. How the disease alters the functional topological organization of the brain, however, remains poorly understood. Furthermore, how levodopa modulates network topology in PD is largely unknown. The objective of this study was to use resting-state functional MRI and graph theory to determine how small-world architecture is altered in PD and affected by levodopa administration. Methods: Twenty-one PD patients and 20 controls underwent functional MRI scanning. PD patients were scanned off medication and 1 hour after 200 mg levodopa. Imaging data were analyzed using 226 nodes comprising 10 intrinsic brain networks. Correlation matrices were generated for each subject and converted into cost-thresholded, binarized adjacency matrices. Cost-integrated whole-brain global and local efficiencies were compared across groups and tested for relationships with disease duration and severity. Results: Data from 2 patients and 4 controls were excluded because of excess motion. Patients off medication showed no significant changes in global efficiency and overall local efficiency, but in a subnetwork analysis did show increased local efficiency in executive (P = 0.006) and salience (P = 0.018) networks. Levodopa significantly decreased local efficiency (P = 0.039) in patients except within the subcortical network, in which it significantly increased local efficiency (P = 0.007). Conclusions: Levodopa modulates global and local efficiency measures of small-world topology in PD, suggesting that degeneration of nigrostriatal neurons in PD may be associated with a large-scale network reorganization and that levodopa tends to normalize the disrupted network topology in PD. © 2016 International Parkinson and Movement Disorder Society.
Article
Emerging research suggests that normative variation in parenting quality relates to children's brain development. However, although the young brain is presumed to be especially sensitive to environmental influence, to our knowledge only two studies have examined parenting quality with infants as it relates to indicators of brain development, and both were cross‐sectional. This longitudinal study investigated whether different components of maternal sensitivity in infancy predicted the volume of two brain structures presumed to be particularly sensitive to early experience, namely the amygdala and the hippocampus. Three dimensions of sensitivity (Cooperation/Attunement, Positivity, Accessibility/Availability) were observed in 33 mother–infant dyads at 1 year of age and children underwent structural magnetic resonance imaging at age 10. Higher maternal Accessibility/Availability during mother–infant interactions was found to be predictive of smaller right amygdala volume, while greater maternal positivity was predictive of smaller bilateral hippocampal volumes. These longitudinal findings extend those of previous cross‐sectional studies and suggest that a multidimensional approach to maternal behavior could be a fruitful way to further advance research in this area, given that different facets of parenting might be differentially predictive of distinct aspects of neurodevelopment.
Article
This study aimed to identify distinct behavioral profiles in a population-based sample of 654 4-year-old children and characterize their relationships with brain functional networks using resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging data. Young children showed 7 behavioral profiles, including a super healthy behavioral profile with the lowest scores across all Child Behavior CheckList (CBCL) subscales (G1) and other 6 behavioral profiles, respectively with pronounced withdrawal (G2), somatic complaints (G3), anxiety and withdrawal (G4), somatic complaints and withdrawal (G5), the mixture of emotion, withdrawal, and aggression (G6), and attention (G7) problems. Compared with children in G1, children with withdrawal shared abnormal functional connectivities among the sensorimotor networks. Children in emotionally relevant problems shared the common pattern among the attentional and frontal networks. Nevertheless, children in sole withdrawal problems showed a unique pattern of connectivity alterations among the sensorimotor, cerebellar, and salience networks. Children with somatic complaints showed abnormal functional connectivities between the attentional and subcortical networks, and between the language and posterior default mode networks. This study provides novel evidence on the existence of behavioral heterogeneity in early childhood and its associations with specific functional networks that are clinically relevant phenotypes for mental illness and are apparent from early childhood.