ArticlePDF Available

Neurobehavioral and Cognitive Performances of Children Exposed to Low-Dose Radiation in the Chernobyl Accident The Israeli Chernobyl Health Effects Study

Authors:
  • israeli academic college

Abstract and Figures

Exposure to low levels of ionizing radiation after the Chernobyl accident in the Ukraine could potentially have influenced the neurobehavioral and cognitive performances of exposed children. A cohort study of adolescents who were children at the time of the accident and who subsequently emigrated to Israel was conducted in 1998-2001. A total of 1,629 children (59% of all 2,769 invited) were included in the study (41% from higher contamination areas, 25% from lower contamination areas, 34% from noncontaminated areas). Mean scores of the Raven Standard Progressive Matrices Test were highest in children in all exposure groups whose parents had a high level of education. No overall relation was found between the cognitive function scores of the child and his/her putative radiation exposure level. Conners' test T scores did not differ significantly by level of exposure. Mothers of all exposure groups who were pregnant at the time of the accident gave their children significantly higher Conners' test scores than did those who were not pregnant. Scores for hyperactivity and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder were significantly higher among those who were in utero at the time of the accident. These results do not show differences of neurobehavioral or cognitive performance in exposed versus nonexposed children. There is a possible behavioral effect among offspring of pregnant mothers or mothers of very young children in all exposure levels.
Content may be subject to copyright.
453
Am J Epidemiol
2004;160:453–459
American Journal of Epidemiology
Copyright © 2004 by the Johns Hopkins Bloomberg School of Public Health
All rights reserved
Vol. 160, No. 5
Printed in U.S.A.
DOI: 10.1093/aje/kwh231
Neurobehavioral and Cognitive Performances of Children Exposed to Low-Dose
Radiation in the Chernobyl Accident
The Israeli Chernobyl Health Effects Study
N. Bar Joseph
1
, D. Reisfeld
1
, E. Tirosh
2,3
, Z. Silman
1
, and G. Rennert
1,3
1
CHS National Cancer Control Center and Department of Community Medicine and Epidemiology, Carmel Medical Center and
Technion S. Neaman Institute, Haifa, Israel.
2
The Hannah Khoushy Child Development Center, Bnai Zion Medical Center, Haifa, Israel.
3
Technion Faculty of Medicine, Haifa, Israel.
Received for publication May 7, 2003; accepted for publication March 19, 2004.
Exposure to low levels of ionizing radiation after the Chernobyl accident in the Ukraine could potentially have
influenced the neurobehavioral and cognitive performances of exposed children. A cohort study of adolescents
who were children at the time of the accident and who subsequently emigrated to Israel was conducted in 1998–
2001. A total of 1,629 children (59% of all 2,769 invited) were included in the study (41% from higher
contamination areas, 25% from lower contamination areas, 34% from noncontaminated areas). Mean scores of
the Raven Standard Progressive Matrices Test were highest in children in all exposure groups whose parents
had a high level of education. No overall relation was found between the cognitive function scores of the child
and his/her putative radiation exposure level. Conners’ test T scores did not differ significantly by level of
exposure. Mothers of all exposure groups who were pregnant at the time of the accident gave their children
significantly higher Conners’ test scores than did those who were not pregnant. Scores for hyperactivity and
attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder were significantly higher among those who were in utero at the time of the
accident. These results do not show differences of neurobehavioral or cognitive performance in exposed versus
nonexposed children. There is a possible behavioral effect among offspring of pregnant mothers or mothers of
very young children in all exposure levels.
accidents, radiation; child; cognition; health; neurobehavioral manifestations; radiation
Abbreviation: ADHD, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder.
The accident at the Chernobyl Nuclear Power Plant in the
Ukraine in 1986 was the largest nuclear reactor accident ever
reported. The accident caused the dispersion of radioactive
fallout over large areas, which exposed millions of people to
different amounts of radiation (1).
Although a lot of attention was first paid to the physical
health consequences of the radiation-exposed groups,
concern was also raised regarding the neurodevelopmental
sequelae in those who were exposed to radiation in utero or
at a young age. It could be hypothesized that prenatal and
early childhood irradiation of the brain could lead to
neurobehavioral dysfunction (2).
Studies of the survivors of the atomic bombs in Hiroshima
and Nagasaki, Japan, indicated that the fetal brain is most
susceptible to high doses of ionizing irradiation for 8–15
weeks after conception. Fetal exposure to high doses of radi-
ation increased the risk of mental retardation, small head
size, subsequent seizures, and poor performance on conven-
tional tests of intelligence (3, 4).
After the Chernobyl accident, an excess of thyroid cancers
among exposed children was reported and, more recently,
some genetic changes of unclear importance have also been
described in children possibly influenced by the Chernobyl
radiation exposure (5, 6). Psychological stress, anxiety, and
fear, especially in adults, were reported in several studies
Correspondence to Dr. Gad Rennert, CHS National Cancer Control Center, Carmel Medical Center, Haifa 34362, Israel (e-mail:
rennert@tx.technion.ac.il).
by guest on June 7, 2013http://aje.oxfordjournals.org/Downloaded from
454 Joseph et al.
Am J Epidemiol
2004;160:453–459
after the Chernobyl accident (7–12). Studies of the impact of
the Three Mile Island accident showed in one study greater
psychological distress among women who were pregnant or
had young children (13) but showed in another study no such
effect (14). Further, young children living near the Three
Mile Island reactor did not differ from children living next to
another reactor in their prevalence of behavioral problems
(15).
In addition to several hundred thousand individuals who
were relocated within the former Soviet Union after the
disaster, hundreds of thousands more left the country. Many
of them emigrated to Israel. Israeli immigrants from the
Chernobyl region manifested high levels of anxiety and
concern about radiation. Additionally, they showed inaccu-
rate preconceptions about the health risk of chronic radiation
exposure and sought extensive health care for what they
feared would be long-term illnesses caused by the Chernobyl
Nuclear Power Plant disaster (16, 17). No differences were
found in the cognitive and neuropsychological functioning
of evacuated children who were in utero or up to 15 months
of age at the time of the accident when they were compared
with their classmates in Kiev (18).
The present study, among others, was designed to evaluate
possible influences of the exposure to ionizing radiation after
the Chernobyl accident on the neurobehavioral and cognitive
performances of Israeli immigrants who were young chil-
dren at the time of the accident.
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Study population
All children aged up to 4 years (including in utero) at the
time of the accident who emigrated to Israel from the Gomel
(highly exposed) region and a convenience sample of immi-
grant children from the Mogilev and Kiev (mildly exposed)
regions and from the (nonexposed) cities of Moscow and St.
Petersburg, as well as other unexposed areas of Belarus,
were invited to participate in the study. Exposure levels of
the various areas were determined according to geographic
cesium contamination maps of the International Atomic
Energy Agency. Altogether, 1,629 children (58.8 percent of
2,769 children sampled and invited, whose mailing
addresses were confirmed from a selected cohort of 4,319
children) who emigrated to Israel between 1989 and 2000
were included. Of these, 667 (41 percent) children came
from areas with high contamination, 408 (25 percent) came
from areas of low contamination, and 554 (34 percent) came
from noncontaminated areas. Compliance rates of samples
of children with an identified address are as follows: 65
percent for children from Moscow-St. Petersburg, 62.2
percent for children from other Belarusian cities, 60.2
percent for children from Mogilev, 57.0 percent for children
from Gomel, and 54.9 percent for children from Kiev.
The area of origin in the former Soviet Union was defined
as the settlement of origin in their home countries recorded
by the Ministry of Immigrant Absorption in Israel upon
registration of their families for immigration. This indicator
was validated by information collected during the partici-
pant’s interview. Final assignment to area of exposure was
based on the true origin of the child and not on the informa-
tion appearing in the official documents.
All the children were invited, with their mothers, to the
CHS National Cancer Control Center at the Carmel Medical
Center in Haifa, Israel. The Israeli Chernobyl Health Effects
Study took place during the years 1998–2001. Participating
children were thus aged 12–18 years at the time of the study.
Data collection
After signing an informed consent form, the child’s
mother was interviewed by trained Russian-speaking inter-
viewers who used a standardized questionnaire. The ques-
tionnaire sought demographic data, residential and personal
radiation exposure (for dose estimation) data, and informa-
tion on exposure to various risk factors during the relevant
pregnancy, disease status, and health behavior patterns of the
relevant child. All children undertook a cognitive test and a
questionnaire evaluating their behavior. A complete physical
examination, self-reported puberty staging according to the
Tanner scale, and venous blood tests including hematology
and thyroid panels were also conducted but are not discussed
in this report. Assessment of attention-deficit/hyperactivity
disorder (ADHD) was carried out using the short Conners’
Rating Scales-Revised for children and their mothers (19).
Nonverbal intelligence was examined by the Raven Standard
Progressive Matrices Test (20). This test is not language
dependent and, therefore, functions well in a new immigrant
population.
Measurement of cognitive ability.
Cognitive ability of the
children was measured by using the Raven Standard
Progressive Matrices Test (1996 edition) (20). This
nonverbal intelligence test was selected because the study
population of immigrant children had diverse levels of
knowledge of Hebrew. The test consists of 60 diagrammatic
puzzles. Each puzzle has one part missing, which the child
has to fill in with one of the options provided after proper
explanation by the test administrator. No time limit was set
for completion of the test.
The child’s score was calculated as the total number of
problems solved correctly. A table of percentile norms (20,
table SPM 14) was used to interpret the scores. These norms
enabled us to compare the performances of children in the
various exposure groups.
Assessment of attention.
The assessment of ADHD was
carried out by using Conners’ Rating Scales-Revised. These
scales are a comprehensive assessment of psychopathology
and problem behaviors of children and adolescents.
The Adolescent Self-report Scales and the Parent Rating
Scales (which relate to the child’s behavior) (19) were
chosen for this study. The short forms (27-item question-
naire) yielded scores on four parameters (appearing in both
the adolescent and parent questionnaires): oppositional prob-
lems, cognitive problems, hyperactivity, and risk of ADHD.
Children were provided with a Hebrew version of the ques-
tionnaire, and their mothers were provided with a Russian
version. Conners’ Rating Scales-Revised are available in
only English, Spanish, and French. Because we needed a
Hebrew version for the children and a Russian version for
their mothers, the questionnaires were translated into
by guest on June 7, 2013http://aje.oxfordjournals.org/Downloaded from
Neurobehavior and Cognition in Radiation-exposed Children 455
Am J Epidemiol
2004;160:453–459
Hebrew and Russian and then translated back into English to
validate the translation. The crude scores of the Conners’ test
were translated into T scores, which are age and gender
specific. T scores of 65 or more usually indicate a significant
clinical problem, while T scores between 56 and 64 warn of
a possible problem.
Statistical analyses
Statistical analyses using SPSS/PC, version 10.07 (SPSS,
Inc., Chicago, Illinois), software included comparison of
different exposure groups (all groups, or highest exposure
group vs. others) by means of analysis of variance. After
studying individual scores, we used models that adjusted the
test scores for the following potential effect modifiers, iden-
tified in the univariate analysis: child’s gender, birth order,
number of siblings, family status (two parents or single
parent), timing of exposure (in utero vs. not in utero), and
parent’s level of education.
RESULTS
Of the 2,769 children approached, 1,629 (58.8 percent)
participated. Of these, 270 (16.6 percent) were in utero at the
time of the accident (table 1). The mean age of the study
participants at the time of the accident in 1986 was 1.48
(standard deviation, 1.31) years. Of the participating chil-
dren, 48 percent were females, and 52 percent were males.
No major differences were found in the ages at the time of
the accident (p = 0.62) and in the gender distribution (p =
0.88) of the children as related to the different exposure
groups. Most children (69 percent) had one sibling, 21
percent had no siblings, and 10 percent had two or more
siblings. Single-parent families comprised 20 percent of all
study families. None of the participating children suffered
from significant mental or physical handicaps at the time of
the study.
The age of the participating children at the time of the
study ranged from 12 to 18 years. About a fourth of the chil-
dren were aged 12–13 years at the time of the test, 45 percent
were aged 14–15 years, and 31 percent were aged 16–18
years.
The exposure groups differed significantly in the level of
education of their parents. Higher levels of education for
both parents were noted in residents of the Moscow-St.
Petersburg group, followed by residents of Kiev and Minsk
(low- or no-exposure group), and were lowest in members of
the high-exposure groups (table 2).
Cognitive assessment by the Raven Standard
Progressive Matrices Test
Mean scores (percentiles) of the Raven Standard Progres-
sive Matrices Test were highest in children in all exposure
groups whose parents had a high level of education. The
mean scores of the matrices test, adjusted for parent’s level
of education, differed significantly (p < 0.0001), although
the difference did not correspond to the level of radiation
exposure (table 3). No other variable was found to signifi-
cantly influence the results. Children who were in utero at
the time of the accident did not score differently from older
children in the same exposure area.
Attention assessment by the Conners’ test
The children’s test results for the Conners’ test did not
show any significant difference by radiation exposure level,
after controlling for gender, birth order, number of siblings,
family status, and parent’s level of education. Rates of all
exposure groups were within a normal range (score of less
than 50) (table 4).
Significant differences in the oppositional and hyperac-
tivity subscale scores of the mother’s test, but not in the
subscale scores for cognitive function and ADHD, were
noticed between the different exposure groups. However,
TABLE 1. Child population, by residency at time of accident, Israeli Chernobyl Health Effects Study, 1998–
2001
* According to an International Atomic Energy Agency map, 1989.
† A total of 26 children (3.9%) lived in very high contaminated areas (15–40 Ci/km
2
).
Residential area
Level of
cesium 137
(Ci/km
2
)*
No. of
children
approached
Participating children
Children in utero
at time of accident
No. % No. %
Gomel region, Belarus, and other
exposed areas in Belarus and the
Ukraine
1–40 1,169 667† 57.0 98 15
Mogilev region, Belarus <1 216 130 60.2 14 11
Kiev region, the Ukraine <1 506 278 54.9 48 17
Other regions in Belarus 0.1 598 372 62.0 72 19
Cities of Moscow and St. Petersburg,
Russia 0.1 280 182 65.0 38 21
Total 2,769 1,629 270 16.6
by guest on June 7, 2013http://aje.oxfordjournals.org/Downloaded from
456 Joseph et al.
Am J Epidemiol
2004;160:453–459
these differences were unrelated to the degree of radiation
exposure (highly exposed areas compared with all other
exposure areas or a comparison of all five exposure levels
separately). Most scores were within the normal range (score
of less than 56), while the other scores were within the
borderline range (score of 56–60) (table 4). Mothers with a
high level of education rated their children significantly
lower (on all scales except for the oppositional subscale)
than did less educated mothers.
Mothers’ scores were significantly higher (p 0.001 for
each of the subscales) than their children’s scores. This was
true for each of the five residence groups. The subjective
assessment of the children’s health status by their mothers
was found to correlate negatively with the mother’s score on
the Conners’ test. In all radiation exposure groups, mothers
who rated their children highest (good to excellent health
status) scored lowest on the Conners’ test (p < 0.001).
Mothers from all radiation exposure groups who were
pregnant at the time of the accident rated their children with
significantly higher Conners’ test scores in three of four
categories than did those who were not pregnant. The scores,
however, were mostly in the normal range (table 5).
A negative correlation was found between the scores of the
Raven matrices and the scores of Conners’ test (mother’s or
child’s score). All correlation coefficients were very low but
statistically significant. The highest correlation coefficients
were found between the Raven score and Conners’ test score
for the cognitive subscale (r = 0.2 for mothers, r = 0.1 for
children; p < 0.0001 for both).
DISCUSSION
No relation was found between exposure to radiation
(proxy estimated by location of residence at the time of the
accident) and performance in tests measuring neurobehav-
ioral and cognitive performance. This result may either
reflect a true lack of relation between radiation and behav-
ioral performance or may be the result of one of numerous
biases. This study was a large, population-based study in
which a whole cohort of children emigrating from exposed
areas in the former Soviet Union was recruited and included
a convenience sample of children from nonexposed areas as
well. The sampling frame came from governmental registra-
tion files and is believed to have a high validity and lack of
TABLE 2. Level of parent’s education, by exposure group, Israeli Chernobyl Health Effects Study, 1998–2001
Exposure group
Mother’s level of education Father’s level of education
9–11 years Technical High 9–11 years Technical High
No. % No. % No. % No. % No. % No. %
Gomel and other exposed areas 91 14 295 44 281 42 174 26 232 35 256 39
Mogilev 8 6 57 44 65 50 27 21 55 42 48 37
Kiev 24 9 99 36 154 56 46 17 92 33 138 50
Other Belarus 26 7 132 36 214 58 51 14 112 30 205 56
Moscow and St. Petersburg 1 0.5 33 18 148 81 7 4 28 16 142 80
Total 150 9 616 38 862 53 305 19 519 32 789 49
TABLE 3. Matrices’ mean scores, by parent’s level of education, Israeli Chernobyl Health Effects Study,
1998–2001
* Numbers in parentheses, standard deviation.
† Adjusted for mother’s education.
Gomel Mogilev Kiev Other Belarus
Moscow and
St. Petersburg
Mother’s education
9–11 years 50 (29)* 52 (23) 61 (30) 46 (25)
Technical school 54 (29) 49 (29) 57 (31) 58 (27) 60 (33)
University 67 (27) 61 (29) 76 (21) 69 (27) 78 (24)
Father’s education
9–11 years 52 (27) 46 (28) 63 (29) 54 (29) 52 (30)
Technical school 56 (30) 57 (29) 63 (28) 58 (27) 58 (31)
University 66 (28) 57 (30) 74 (24) 69 (26) 78 (24)
Total† 57 (29) 52 (28) 64 (29) 58 (30) 64 (30)
by guest on June 7, 2013http://aje.oxfordjournals.org/Downloaded from
Neurobehavior and Cognition in Radiation-exposed Children 457
Am J Epidemiol
2004;160:453–459
selection bias. Compliance in this study was about 60
percent and was nondifferentially distributed among the
various exposure groups. This, most probably, reflects a
nonattendance that is not disease status related. If sick chil-
dren were more apt to participate in this study, and if disease
in these children was related to radiation exposure, then the
data would be biased against the null. Because we did not
find a relation between residence in areas affected by the
Chernobyl accident and the degree of neurobehavioral or
cognitive performance in our study population, we assume
that no such bias occurred. In addition, the health status of
the participating children was not found to differ between the
cases and the controls in our series. Another possible bias is
a misclassification bias in which children from seemingly
exposed areas would be counted as nonexposed and vice
versa. We tested the residence registration in the sampling
frame and found it to correlate fully with the actual place of
residence in 1986. A more serious question is whether resi-
dence in Gomel truly corresponds with high radiation expo-
sure. Data from the former Soviet Union and from maps
produced by the International Atomic Energy Agency
several years after the accident have pointed at the Gomel
region (or oblast) as the area with the highest contamination
level outside the 30-km zone. Nevertheless, it is believed
that urban dwellers as a group were exposed to lower radia-
tion levels than were rural dwellers. Jews in the former
Soviet Union were mostly urban dwellers. This fact, coupled
with evidence that the Chernobyl accident as a whole was a
low-radiation-exposure event, could lead to studying and
comparing groups that do not differ enough between them-
selves to show an increased risk, if such risk actually exists.
No good measure of individual exposure to radiation is
available.
Other findings in the study may indirectly validate the
study results. The educational level of the parents was found,
as expected, to be a strong positive predictor of performance.
Good correlations between cognitive parameters in both
tests and higher Conners’ test scores of mothers versus their
children correspond to the current knowledge in this field.
Interestingly, the neurobehavioral assessment of ADHD
showed that mothers who were pregnant at the time of the
accident scored their children with significantly higher
scores than did nonpregnant women for their children. This
was shown without any relation to exposure. Children’s
assessment scores, in the hyperactivity and the ADHD index,
were significantly higher among those who were in utero at
the time of the accident. This too was unrelated to exposure
level.
Our results may suggest that mothers who were pregnant
or had very young children at the time of the accident,
TABLE 4. Conners’ mean scores adjusted for mother’s education, Israeli Chernobyl Health Effects Study,
1998–2001
* Numbers in parentheses, standard deviation.
† ADHD, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder.
Gomel Mogilev Kiev Other Belarus
Moscow and
St. Petersburg
Child’s scores
Oppositional 48 (6)* 48 (7) 48 (5) 48 (7) 49 (6)
Cognitive 47 (7) 48 (8) 47 (7) 46 (6) 47 (7)
Hyperactivity 46 (7) 46 (7) 45 (7) 45 (7) 45 (7)
ADHD† 47 (8) 48 (9) 46 (7) 47 (7) 47 (7)
Mother’s scores
Oppositional 53 (10) 55 (12) 54 (10) 55 (10) 58 (11)
Cognitive 51 (8) 52 (8) 52 (7) 52 (8) 53 (8)
Hyperactivity 57 (13) 61 (16) 58 (14) 58 (13) 61 (13)
ADHD 51 (8) 52 (9) 52 (8) 52 (7) 54 (8)
TABLE 5. Mean item score in Conners’ test, by pregnancy status at time of accident, Israeli Chernobyl
Health Effects Study, 1998–2001
* Numbers in parentheses, standard deviation.
† ADHD, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder.
Conners’ test item
Pregnant
(
n
= 270)
Not pregnant
(
n
= 1,346)
p
value
A (oppositional) 55.23 (10.28)* 53.77 (10.00) 0.029
B (cognitive problems) 51.61 (7.60) 50.81 (7.65) 0.119
C (hyperactivity) 56.92 (13.72) 56.82 (13.44) 0.002
D (risk of ADHD†) 52.44 (7.80) 51.36 (7.74) 0.036
by guest on June 7, 2013http://aje.oxfordjournals.org/Downloaded from
458 Joseph et al.
Am J Epidemiol
2004;160:453–459
regardless of their location in the former Soviet Union, had
higher levels of anxiety and fear of the radiation. This might
stem from lack of information and inaccurate preconceptions
about the exact magnitude and spatial distribution of radia-
tion. Such a sense of fear might either distort their perception
or, alternatively, have been transmitted to their children who
showed higher hyperactivity and ADHD index scores among
the younger ages.
Similar results were found by Ginzburg (7), who reported
high levels of anxiety and stress among adults from contam-
inated and noncontaminated villages. Nearly half of this
study population, regardless of the true level of exposure,
were unsure if they had a radiation-related illness. Two other
studies from the Chernobyl area reported a high risk of
psychiatric disorders among women with children under the
age of 18 years residing in the exposed areas (21, 22). A
study on the psychological effects of the Three Mile Island
accident showed that women who were pregnant or had
young children experienced the greatest psychological
distress after the accident (13). The trauma brought on by the
Chernobyl accident, experienced by mothers, was reflected
in another study in their perceptions of their children’s well
being, particularly somatic symptoms. This anxiety,
however, was not transmitted to the children themselves (8).
Two previous studies of the Chernobyl accident suggested
lower cognitive performance among children exposed prena-
tally when compared with nonexposed children (2, 23). One
of these studies (2) included evacuated children who were
described as having been significantly more exposed than
our study population. The second study (23) related to prena-
tally exposed children only, and it was based on very small
numbers of children. Furthermore, these studies did not
control for possible confounders (mainly, level of parent’s
education) as did our study, which did not demonstrate a
similar lower performance. It is possible that emigration to a
new place, remote from the irradiated area, played a role in
lowering the anxiety level in the family.
We could not evaluate what, if any, influence the site of
residence in Israel had. Moreover, we could also not deter-
mine if the level of education received by these children in
Israel had an effect. However, because of Israel’s manda-
tory-education law, it highly likely that all these young chil-
dren received formal education in Israeli schools. Given the
small size of Israel and given that most of our participants
came from one of nine cities in the country, it is unlikely that
residence in Israel biased our results.
In a comparison of the cognitive and neuropsychological
functioning of children who were between in utero and up to
15 months of age at the time of the Chernobyl disaster and
who were evacuated to Kiev and their Kiev school class-
mates, no difference in performance was found (18).
Self-assessment of the child’s health by the mothers was,
as expected, negatively correlated with the attention assess-
ment scores of the child. Health self-assessment has been
found in many studies to correlate positively with physical
health status (24).
In conclusion, the results of our large, population-based
study of children who had been exposed to low-dose
ionizing radiation do not show differences in neurobehav-
ioral or cognitive performance compared with results of
nonexposed children. Thus, low-radiation events probably
do not result in deviations in neurobehavioral and cognitive
performance. Lower neurobehavioral performance of chil-
dren who were in utero or young at the time of the accident,
in all exposure groups, may possibly hint at a broader,
country-wide, Chernobyl-related effect resulting in
increased levels of anxiety in the pregnant mothers or
mothers of very young children in the former Soviet Union.
Attention should be paid by agencies caring for relevant
former Soviet Union populations to the possible effects of
the anxiety of their subjects on the behavioral and cognitive
functioning of the children. Providing validated information
with regard to the true health effects of the Chernobyl acci-
dent may help to lower the level of anxiety.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
This study was part of the Israeli Chernobyl Effects Study
supported by a grant from the US Agency for International
Development, US State Department. The study was operated
through the S. Neaman Institute at the Technion-Israel Insti-
tute of Technology.
The authors would like to thank the pediatricians Drs.
Irena Chistyakov, Marina Umansky, and Leonid Ganelis and
interviewers Tanya Agranovsky and Yael Aerov for their
help in carrying out the study.
REFERENCES
1. Ilyin LA. Experience from Chernobyl in the context of current
problems of radiation. In: Radiation protection in nuclear
energy: proceedings of an international conference on radiation
protection in nuclear energy. Vienna, Austria: International
Atomic Energy Agency, 1988.
2. Nyagu AI, Loganovsky KN, Loganovskaja TK. Psychophysio-
logic aftereffects of prenatal irradiation. Int J Psychophysiol
1998;30:303–11.
3. Yamazaki JN, Schull WJ. Perinatal loss and neurological
abnormalities among children of the atomic bomb. Nagasaki
and Hiroshima revisited, 1949 to 1989. JAMA 1990;264:605–
9.
4. Otake M, Shull WJ. In utero exposure to A-bomb radiation and
mental retardation; a reassessment. Br J Radiol 1984;57:409–
14.
5. Weinberg HS, Nevo E, Korol A, et al. Molecular changes in the
offspring of liquidators who emigrated to Israel from the Cher-
nobyl disaster area. Environ Health Perspect 1997;105(suppl
6):1479–81.
6. Weinberg HS, Korol AB, Kirzhner VM, et al. Very high muta-
tion rate in offspring of Chernobyl accident liquidators. Proc R
Soc Lond B Biol Sci 2001;268:1001–5.
7. Ginzburg HM. The psychological consequences of the Cherno-
byl accident: findings from the International Atomic Agency
Study. Public Health Rep 1993;108:184–92.
8. Bromet EJ, Goldgaber D, Carlson G, et al. Children’s well-
being 11 years after the Chernobyl catastrophe. Arch Gen Psy-
chiatry 2000;57:563–71.
9. Cwikel J, Rozovski U. Coping with the stress of immigration
among new immigrants to Israel from Commonwealth of Inde-
pendent States (CIS) who were exposed to Chernobyl: the
by guest on June 7, 2013http://aje.oxfordjournals.org/Downloaded from
Neurobehavior and Cognition in Radiation-exposed Children 459
Am J Epidemiol
2004;160:453–459
effect of age. Int J Aging Hum Dev 1998;46:305–18.
10. Yevelson II, Abdelgani A, Cwikel J, et al. Bridging the gap in
mental health approaches between East and West: the psycho-
social consequences of radiation exposure. Environ Health Per-
spect 1997;105(suppl 6):1551–6.
11. Cwikel J, Abdelgani A, Goldsmith JR, et al. Two-year follow
up study of stress-related disorders among immigrants to Israel
from the Chernobyl area. Environ Health Perspect 1997;
105(suppl 6):1545–50.
12. Zilber N, Lerner Y. Psychological distress among recent immi-
grants from the former Soviet Union to Israel. I. Correlates of
level of distress. Psychol Med 1996;26:493–501.
13. Houts PS, Cleary PD, Hu TW. The Three Mile Island crisis:
psychological, social, and economic impacts on the surround-
ing population. University Park, PA: Pennsylvania State Uni-
versity Press, 1988.
14. Houts PS, Tokuhata GK, Bratz J, et al. Effect of pregnancy dur-
ing TMI crisis on mothers’ mental health and their child’s
development. Am J Public Health 1991;81:384–6.
15. Cornely P, Bromet E. Prevalence of behavior problems in three
year old children living near Three Mile Island: a comparative
analysis. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 1986;27:489–98.
16. Weinberg AD, Kripalani S, McCarthy PL, et al. Caring for sur-
vivors of the Chernobyl disaster: what the clinician should
know. JAMA 1995;274:408–12.
17. Remennick LI. Immigrants from Chernobyl-affected areas in
Israel: the link between health and social adjustment. Soc Sci
Med 2002;54:309–17.
18. Litcher L, Bromet EJ, Carlson G, et al. School and neuropsy-
chological performance of evacuated children in Kiev 11 years
after the Chernobyl disaster. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2000;
41:291–9.
19. Conners CK. Conners’ Rating Scales-Revised: technical man-
ual. New York, NY: Multi-Health Systems, 1997.
20. Raven JC, Court JH, Raven J. Standard progressive matrices.
San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation, 1996:50–66.
21. Havenaar JM, Van den Brink W, Van den Bout J, et al. Mental
health problems in the Gomel region (Belarus): an analysis of
risk factors in an area affected by the Chernobyl disaster.
Psychol Med 1996;26:845–55.
22. Havenaar JM, Rumyantzeva GM, Van den Brink W, et al. Long
term mental health effects of the Chernobyl disaster: an epide-
miologic survey in two former Soviet regions. Am J Psychiatry
1997;154:1605–7.
23. Kolominsky Y, Igumnov S, Drozdovitch V. The psychological
development of children from Belarus exposed in the prenatal
period to radiation from the Chernobyl atomic power plant. J
Child Psychol Psychiatry 1999;40:299–305.
24. Mossey JM, Shapiro E. Self rated health: a predictor of mortal-
ity among the elderly. Am J Public Health 1982;72:800–6.
by guest on June 7, 2013http://aje.oxfordjournals.org/Downloaded from
... Only after the Chernobyl incident in 1986 did the scientific community begin to study the effects of accidental exposure to ionizing radiation on cognitive functioning (see Bromet, Havenaar, & Guey, 2011 for a detailed review). The majority of research has focused on three specific groups: children that were exposed to radiation in utero or as infants (e.g., Bromet et al., 2011;Hall et al., 2004;Joseph, Reisfeld, Tirosh, Silman, & Rennert, 2004;Loganovsky, 2009;Schull & Otake, 1999), the cleanup workers responsible for site-remediation after nuclear accidents (e.g., Bromet et al., 2011;Gamache, Levinson, Reeves, Bidyuk, & Brantley, 2005), and human populations who lived in close proximity to the immediate area of the accident (e.g., Bromet et al., 2011;Gamache et al., 2005;Joseph et al., 2004). These studies have found that children and adults exposed to radiation exhibit long-term symptoms including a loss of mental power, reduced efficiency, and psychomotor slowing (Gamache et al., 2005;Hall et al., 2004). ...
... Only after the Chernobyl incident in 1986 did the scientific community begin to study the effects of accidental exposure to ionizing radiation on cognitive functioning (see Bromet, Havenaar, & Guey, 2011 for a detailed review). The majority of research has focused on three specific groups: children that were exposed to radiation in utero or as infants (e.g., Bromet et al., 2011;Hall et al., 2004;Joseph, Reisfeld, Tirosh, Silman, & Rennert, 2004;Loganovsky, 2009;Schull & Otake, 1999), the cleanup workers responsible for site-remediation after nuclear accidents (e.g., Bromet et al., 2011;Gamache, Levinson, Reeves, Bidyuk, & Brantley, 2005), and human populations who lived in close proximity to the immediate area of the accident (e.g., Bromet et al., 2011;Gamache et al., 2005;Joseph et al., 2004). These studies have found that children and adults exposed to radiation exhibit long-term symptoms including a loss of mental power, reduced efficiency, and psychomotor slowing (Gamache et al., 2005;Hall et al., 2004). ...
... After the Chernobyl nuclear power plant accident, studies in several countries reported intellectual impairment, a tendency toward behavioral and emotional disorders, lower IQ, and poorer school outcomes in prenatally exposed children compared to controls (3)(4)(5) while other studies found no relationship between cognitive function in children and putative radiation exposures (6,7). Decreased verbal memory and executive functioning were reported in prenatally exposed adolescents (8), as were decreased full-scale IQ and verbal IQ scores in adolescents exposed to low-dose radiation before gestational week 16 (9). ...
Article
Although some adverse effects on neurocognitive function have been reported in children and adolescents irradiated prenatally during the atomic bombings and the Chernobyl nuclear accident, little information is available for effects on the elderly. Here we evaluate the effects of prenatal exposure to atomic bomb radiation on subjective neurocognitive function in aged survivors. To evaluate neurocognitive impairment, we mailed the Neurocognitive Questionnaire (NCQ), a self-administered scale, to prenatally exposed survivors, including clinic visitors and non-visitors at the time of the 2011 and 2013 Adult Health Study (AHS) examinations. We received replies from 444 individuals (mean age, 66.9 ± 0.6 years). After adjusting for sex, city, and educational background, we found no significant effects of radiation, clinic visit, or interaction between radiation and clinic visit on the scores of the 4 NCQ factors of metacognition, emotional regulation, motivation/organization, and processing speed. Even in analyses considering gestational age at the time of the bombings, none of the 4 NCQ factor scores was related to maternal uterine dose. There remains the limitation that this study consisted of healthy survivors, but we found no significant radiation effect on late-life cognition in people prenatally exposed to atomic bomb radiation.
... Previously published studies have shown an association between low dose prenatal radiation exposure and some cognitive functioning; mainly schizophrenia and verbal IQ. 44,45 However, consequent and more recent studies were not consistent with these findings. 46,47 Two comprehensive studies in Sweden and Norway collected exposure data following the Chernobyl accident in April 1986 combined with population-based registries to assess longterm effects of foetal exposure on neurodevelopmental outcomes. They found no evidence that radioactive fallout from Chernobyl in Norway, was associated with serious neurodevelopmental conditions (cerebral palsy, mental retardation, schizophrenia, epilepsy, or severe hearing and vision problems). ...
Article
Full-text available
There are natural concerns regarding the risks posed to the foetus by ionizing radiation exposure during pregnancy. Therefore, many female physicians select to avoid working in an environment associated with ionizing radiation exposure like the catheterization laboratory and even exclude training as electrophysiology, interventional cardiologists, or radiologists. For those already working in this field, pregnancy involves usually a 1-year interruption (pregnancy and maternity leave) to their careers, leading at times to delays in the decision to become pregnant. This review describes the low added risk of malformation/cancer in the offspring, highlight gaps in our understanding, discuss several common wrong beliefs, and recommend how to further decrease radiation dose, especially during pregnancy.
... Although there are no studies relating the cancer risk due to radiation exposure in premature babies and there is no recommended safe level of radiation in this population it is well known that in-utero exposure is associated with an increased cancer risk 11 being the maximum allowable exposure during pregnancy 1 mSV. 1,12 None of the patients from our study exceed this dose due the use of digital radiography being the cumulative effective doses from the first period of our study similar to others previously reported. ...
Article
Objective To reduce radiation exposure in newborns admitted due respiratory distress based on the implementation of lung ultrasound (LUS). Design Quality improvement (QI), prospective, before-after, pilot study. Setting Third level neonatal intensive care unit (NICU) level with 25-bed and 1800 deliveries/year. Patients Inclusion criteria were neonates admitted with respiratory distress. Interventions After a theoretical and practical LUS training a new protocol was approved and introduced to the unit were LUS was the first-line image. To study the effect of the intervention we compare two 6-month periods: group 1, with the previous chest X-ray (CXR)-protocol (CXR as the first diagnostic technique) vs. group 2, once LUS-protocol had been implemented. Main variables of interest The main QI measures were the total exposure to radiation. Secondary QI were to evaluate if the LUS protocol modified the clinical evolution as well as the frequency of complications. Results 122 patients were included. The number of CXR was inferior in group 2 (group 1: 2 CXR (IQR 1–3) vs. Group 2: 0 (IQR 0–1), p < 0.001), as well as had lower median radiation per baby which received at least one CXR: 56 iGy (IQR 32–90) vs. 30 iGy (IQR 30–32), p < 0.001. Respiratory support was similar in both groups, with lower duration of non-invasive mechanical ventilation and oxygen duration the second group (p < 0.05). No differences regarding respiratory development complications, length of stay and mortality were found. Conclusions The introduction of LUS protocol in unit decreases the exposure radiation in infants without side effects.
... 10 The first is related to the extension of the disaster that causes deaths, morbidity, destruction, environmental adversities, and evacuation of communities. [11][12][13][14][15][16][17] The second group is associated with post-disaster circumstances that may alleviate or exacerbate the socioeconomic, health, and environmental impacts. The ability of the responsible actors in providing prompt emotional and financial support is crucial for the healing process of the affected communities. ...
Article
Objectives In 2015, a dam collapsed at Samarco iron ore mine in the municipality of Mariana, Brazil, and contaminated more than 600 km of watercourses and destroyed almost 1600 acres of vegetation. Nineteen people died and more than 600 families lost their homes. This study aimed to estimate health-related quality of life (HRQoL) losses owing to this disaster. Methods We collected data from a probabilistic sample of 459 individuals aged 15 years or older. Household face-to-face interviews were conducted in December 2018. Pre-event data were not available for this population, so respondents were asked to evaluate at present and in retrospect their health status using EQ-5D-3L. The Minas Gerais societal value sets for EQ-5D-3L health preferences, estimated in 2011, were used to calculate utility losses. The health loss estimation from EQ-5D will form the basis for the calculation of compensation payments for the victims. Results Approximately 74% of the study population suffered some HRQoL loss. On average, EQ-5D index values decreased from 0.95 to 0.76. The greatest effects were observed for the anxiety/depression dimension, followed by pain/discomfort. Before the tragedy, the proportion of individuals with severe anxiety/depression and pain/discomfort was equal to 1% rising to 23% and 11%, respectively. Conclusions Catastrophic losses owing to the Samarco disaster were found. The EQ-5D-3L instrument showed feasibility and sensitiveness to measure HRQoL losses owing to a negative health shock in a low-income Brazilian population.
... In particular, Bromet et al. pointed out issues regarding the mental health status of children 10) . Other studies have also reported on the mental health status and the neurobehavioral and cognitive performances of mothers with young children 11,12) . Ongoing investigations continue to focus on the problem of health effects, including mental health effects, in children after the Chernobyl nuclear accident 13) . ...
Article
Full-text available
Introduction:We evaluated the mental health status of children residing in Kawauchi village (Kawauchi), Fukushima Prefecture, after the 2011 accident at the Fukushima Daiichi Nuclear Power Station, based on the children’s experience of the nuclear disaster. Methods:We conducted this cross-sectional study within the framework of the Fukushima Health Management Survey (FHMS);FHMS data on age, sex, exercise habits, sleeping times, experience of the nuclear disaster, and the “Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ)” scores for 156 children from Kawauchi in 2012 were collected. Groups with and without experience of the nuclear disaster — “nuclear disaster (+)” and “nuclear disaster (−)” — were also compared. Results:Our effective response was 93 (59.6%);the mean SDQ score was 11.4±6.8 among elementary school-aged participants and 12.4±6.8 among junior high school-aged ones. We statistically compared the Total Difficulties Scores (TDS) and sub-item scores of the SDQ between “elementary school” and “junior high school” or “nuclear disaster” (+) and (−). There was no significant difference between these items. Conclusions:We found indications of poor mental health among elementary and junior high school-aged children in the disaster area immediately following the accident, but no differences based on their experience of the nuclear disaster. These results indicate the possibility of triggering stress, separate to that from experiences related to the nuclear disaster, in children who lived in affected rural areas and were evacuated just after the nuclear disaster.
... A follow up study also did not identify any detriment at 19 years of age in individuals exposed in utero to age 15 months (Taormina et al. 2008). Similarly, other studies observed no effect of early childhood low-dose exposure (proxy estimation by residence location at time of accident; in utero and up to 4 years of age) on adolescent cognitive functioning (Joseph et al. 2004). ...
Article
Full-text available
Purpose Exposure to ionizing radiation following environmental contamination (e.g., the Chernobyl and Fukushima nuclear accidents), radiotherapy and diagnostics, occupational roles and space travel has been identified as a possible risk-factor for cognitive dysfunction. The deleterious effects of high doses (≥1.0 Gy) on cognitive functioning are fairly well-understood, while the consequences of low (≤0.1 Gy) and moderate doses (0.1–1.0 Gy) have been receiving more research interest over the past decade. In addition to any impact of actual exposure on cognitive functioning, the persistent psychological stress arising from perceived exposure, particularly following nuclear accidents, may itself impact cognitive functioning. In this review we offer a novel interdisciplinary stance on the cognitive impact of radiation exposure, considering psychological and epidemiological observations of different exposure scenarios such as atomic bombings, nuclear accidents, occupational and medical exposures while accounting for differences in dose, rate of exposure and exposure type. The purpose is to address the question that perceived radiation exposure - even where the actual absorbed dose is 0.0 Gy above background dose - can result in psychological stress, which could in turn lead to cognitive dysfunction. In addition, we highlight the interplay between the mechanisms of perceived exposure (i.e., stress) and actual exposure (i.e., radiation-induced cellular damage), in the generation of radiation-induced cognitive dysfunction. In all, we offer a comprehensive and objective review addressing the potential for cognitive defects in the context of low- and moderate-dose IR exposures. Conclusions Overall the evidence shows prenatal exposure to low and moderate doses to be detrimental to brain development and subsequent cognitive functioning, however the evidence for adolescent and adult low- and moderate-dose exposure remains uncertain. The persistent psychological stress following accidental exposure to low-doses in adulthood may pose a greater threat to our cognitive functioning. Indeed, the psychological implications for instructed cohorts (e.g., astronauts and radiotherapy patients) is less clear and warrants further investigation. Nonetheless, the psychosocial consequences of low- and moderate-dose exposure must be carefully considered when evaluating radiation effects on cognitive functioning, and to avoid unnecessary harm when planning public health response strategies.
Article
Full-text available
Recent international developments have brought a broader awareness of the vulnerability to global peace of the increasing likelihood of nuclear events. The increased nuclear threats from Russia in relation to the current Ukraine war have justifiably raised concerns for nuclear war potential with the U.S. and NATO, and the terrifying large-scale outcomes for populations worldwide. Recent surveys have indicated that 75-80% of people surveyed worldwide, and consistently across nations, believe that nuclear war is more likely now than in the recent past. ¹ Indeed, the fear of nuclear conflict on such a global scale has resulted in a lack of recognition of what is generally recognized as the far greater likelihood of relatively smaller nuclear detonations, at least in the near term . ² Since nuclear detonations have been considered as high impact but low likelihood events in emergency preparedness, there has subsequently been little emphasis on preparing for them, whether large- or small-scale nuclear events. In summary, we therefore find ourselves in the current environment where there is greatly increased awareness of a nuclear threat, though the fear predominates for the less probable large-scale rather than the far more likely small-scale nuclear events, and there is frankly little preparedness for any nuclear event.
Article
Objective To reduce radiation exposure in newborns admitted due respiratory distress based on the implementation of lung ultrasound (LUS). Design Quality improvement (QI), prospective, before-after, pilot study. Setting Third level neonatal intensive care unit (NICU) level with 25-bed and 1800 deliveries/year. Patients Inclusion criteria were neonates admitted with respiratory distress. Interventions After a theoretical and practical LUS training a new protocol was approved and introduced to the unit were LUS was the first-line image. To study the effect of the intervention we compare two 6-month periods: group 1, with the previous chest X-ray (CXR)-protocol (CXR as the first diagnostic technique) vs. group 2, once LUS-protocol had been implemented. Main variables of interest The main QI measures were the total exposure to radiation. Secondary QI were to evaluate if the LUS protocol modified the clinical evolution as well as the frequency of complications. Results 122 patients were included. The number of CXR was inferior in group 2 (group 1: 2 CXR (IQR 1–3) vs. Group 2: 0 (IQR 0–1), p < 0.001), as well as had lower median radiation per baby which received at least one CXR: 56 iGy (IQR 32–90) vs. 30 iGy (IQR 30–32), p < 0.001. Respiratory support was similar in both groups, with lower duration of non-invasive mechanical ventilation and oxygen duration the second group (p < 0.05). No differences regarding respiratory development complications, length of stay and mortality were found. Conclusions The introduction of LUS protocol in unit decreases the exposure radiation in infants without side effects.
Article
Full-text available
The last decades have seen increased concern about the possible effects of low to moderate doses of ionizing radiation (IR) exposure on cognitive function. An interdisciplinary group of experts (biologists, epidemiologists, dosimetrists and clinicians) in this field gathered together in the framework of the European MELODI workshop on non-cancer effects of IR to summarise the state of knowledge on the topic and elaborate research recommendations for future studies in this area. Overall, there is evidence of cognitive effects from low IR doses both from biology and epidemiology, though a better characterization of effects and understanding of mechanisms is needed. There is a need to better describe the specific cognitive function or diseases that may be affected by radiation exposure. Such cognitive deficit characterization should consider the human life span, as effects might differ with age at exposure and at outcome assessment. Measurements of biomarkers, including imaging, will likely help our understanding on the mechanism of cognitive-related radiation induced deficit. The identification of loci of individual genetic susceptibility and the study of gene expression may help identify individuals at higher risk. The mechanisms behind the radiation induced cognitive effects are not clear and are likely to involve several biological pathways and different cell types. Well conducted research in large epidemiological cohorts and experimental studies in appropriate animal models are needed to improve the understanding of radiation-induced cognitive effects. Results may then be translated into recommendations for clinical radiation oncology and imaging decision making processes.
Article
Background: The psychological effects of technological disasters have rarely been studied in children. This study assessed the aftermath of the 1986 Chornobyl disaster in children evacuated to Kyiv from the contaminated zone surrounding the nuclear power facility. Methods: In 1997, we evaluated three hundred 10- to 12-year-old children in Kyiv who were in utero or infants at the time of the disaster and who had resided near Chornobyl (evacuees) and 300 sex-matched homeroom classmates who had never lived in a radiation-contaminated area. Response rates were 92% (evacuees) and 85% (classmates). Data were obtained from children, mothers, and teachers using standard measures of well-being and risk factors for childhood psychopathology. The children also received physical examinations and basic blood tests. ety symptoms and perceived scholastic competence. No differences were found on the Iowa Conners’ Teacher Rating Scale. Although the physical examination and blood test results were normal, the evacuee mothers rated their children’s well-being as significantly worse, especially with respect to somatic symptoms on the Children’s Somatization Inventory and Child Behavior Checklist. The most important risk factors for these ratings were maternal somatization and Chornobyl-related stress. Conclusions: Given the multiple stressful experiences to which evacuee families were exposed, the small differences in the children’s self-reports suggest that there are protective factors in the lives of these children. The trauma experienced by the mothers was reflected in their perceptions of their children’s well-being, particularly somatic symptoms, but was not transmitted to the children themselves. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2000;57:563-571
Book
Since the March 1979 accident at the Three Mile Island (TMI) nuclear power plant, many studies have assessed its impacts. Compiled and summarized in this book are the results of five related surveys, all aimed at the scientific assessment of the psycho-socio-economic behavior of the residents around the TMI facility. These studies are based on a randomly selected, large sample of the population (with telephones) around TMI.
Article
Progressive matrices provide a nonverbal series of tests designed for measuring intelligence. The individual test was standardized on 660 children from Ipswich sampled from those born between 1924 and 1932. Subsequently, 1407 children from the same schools were given group tests. Score values are presented in the form of separate curves for the 5, 10, 25, 50, 75, 90, and 95th percentile points at half-yearly ages from 6 to 14 for the individual test and from 8 to 14 for the group test. The group test also includes percentile values for 3665 male adults. Results are compared with those of the revised Stanford Binet, but no correlations are stated. Case notes show that verbal fluency sometimes influences Binet IQ's, while not influencing matrix test scores. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Abstract The Behavior Screening Questionnaire (BSQ) was used to determine whether 21/2–31/2 yr old children living near the TMI nuclear reactor were more disturbed than children living near another nuclear plant or near a fossil-fuel facility in Pennsylvania when assessed 21/2 yr later. The prevalence of behavior problems was 11%. Differences among the sites in overall rates and individual symptoms were small. Perceptions of environmental stress among the TMI sample of mothers were unrelated to BSQ scores, whereas in the comparison sites, where unemployment was rising, economic concerns were meaningfully related to the BSQ.
Article
This paper examines the cognitive and neuropsychological functioning of children who were in utero to age 15 months at the time of the Chornobyl disaster and were evacuated to Kyiv from the 30-kilometer zone surrounding the plant. Specifically, we compared 300 evacuee children at ages 10–12 with 300 non-evacuee Kyiv classmates on objective and subjective measures of attention, memory, and school performance. The evacuee children were not significantly different from their classmates on the objective measures (grades; Symbolic Relations subtest of the Detroit Test; forms 1 and 2 of the Visual Search and Attention Test; Benton Form A; Trails A; Underline the Words Test) or on most of the subjective measures (the attention subscale of the Child Behavior Checklist completed by mothers; the attention items of the Iowa Conners Teacher's Rating Scale; mother and child perceptions of school performance). The one exception was that 31.3% of evacuee mothers compared to 7.4 %of classmate mothers indicated that their child had a memory problem. However, this subjective measure of memory problems was not significantly related to neuropsychological or school performance. No significant differences were found in comparisons of evacuees and classmates who were in utero at the time of the explosion, children from Pripyat vs. other villages in the 30-kilometer zone, and children manifesting greater generalized anxiety. For both groups, children with greater Chornobyl-focused anxiety performed significantly worse than children with less Chornobyl-focused anxiety on measures of attention. The results thus fail to confirm two previous reports that relatively more children from areas contaminated by radiation had cognitive deficits compared to controls. Possible reasons for the differences in findings among the studies are discussed.
Article
Five years after the Three Mile Island nuclear accident, the mental health of women who had been pregnant and living within 10 miles of Three Mile Island at the time of the accident was similar to that of women from the same area who became pregnant after the accident. Ratings of the development of the two groups of children when they were 5 years old were also similar. However, women who were pregnant during the crisis and had been "extremely disturbed" about their pregnancies rated their children's health as poorer than did the women who were pregnant later.
Article
Studies of the survivors of the atomic bombing of Hiroshima and Nagasaki who were exposed to ionizing radiation in utero have demonstrated a significant increase in perinatal loss and the vulnerability of the developing fetal brain to injury. These studies have also helped to define the stages in the development of the human brain that are particularly susceptible to radiation-related damage. Exposure at critical junctures in development increases the risk of mental retardation, small head size, subsequent seizures, and poor performance on conventional tests of intelligence and in school. The most critical period, 8 through 15 weeks after fertilization, corresponds to that time in development when neuronal production increases and migration of immature neurons to their cortical sites of function occurs. The epidemiologic data are, however, too sparse to settle unequivocally the nature of the dose-response function and, in particular, whether there is or is not a threshold to damage. If a threshold does exist, it appears to be in the 0.10- to 0.20-Gy fetal-dose range in this vulnerable gestational period.
Article
The prevalence of mental retardation in children exposed in utero to the atomic bombs in Hiroshima and Nagasaki has been re-evaluated in reference to gestational age and tissue dose in the fetus. There was no risk at 0-8 weeks post-conception. The highest risk of forebrain damage occurred at 8-15 weeks of gestational age, the time when the most rapid proliferation of neuronal elements and when most, if not all, neuroblast migration to the cerebral cortex from the proliferative zones is occurring. Overall, the risk is five or more times greater in these weeks than in subsequent ones. In the critical period, damage expressed as the frequency of subsequent mental retardation appears to be linearly related to the dose received by the fetus. A linear model is not equally applicable to radiation-related mental retardation after the 15th week, the observed values suggesting that there a threshold may exist. The data are consistent with a probability of occurrence of mental retardation of 0.40% per cGy or 40% per gray.
Article
Data from the Manitoba Longitudinal Study on Aging (MLSA) were used to test the hypothesis that self-rated health (SRH) is a predictor of mortality independent of "objective health status" (OHS). Subjects were a random sample of non-institutionalized residents of Manitoba aged 65+ in 1971 (n = 3,128). A single item measure of SRH was obtained during a survey conducted in 1971; a baseline measure of OHS was derived from physician and self-reported conditions and health service utilization data. Occurrence and date of death during the years 1971-1977 were known. Analyses of the data revealed that, controlling for OHS, age, sex, life satisfaction, income and urban/rural residence, the risk of early mortality (1971-1973) and late mortality (1974-1977) for persons whose SRH was poor was 2.92 and 2.77 times that of those whose SRH was excellent. This increased risk of death associated with poor self-rated health was greater than that associated with poor OHS, poor life satisfaction, low income and being male. These findings provide empirical support for the long held, but inadequately substantiated, belief that the way a person views his health is importantly related to subsequent health outcomes.