ArticlePDF Available

Effects of preferred retinal locus placement on text navigation and development of advantageous trained retinal locus

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Sixty readers were evaluated for visual function and text-navigation ability. The visual field and preferred retinal locus (PRL) were measured with a scanning laser ophthalmoscope (SLO). We found significant differences in text-navigation ability based on scotoma and PRL placement. Readers with a PRL to the left of or above a scotoma had significantly less text-navigation abilities. Readers with a PRL to the left of a scotoma tended to misread words with similar beginnings and omit the last word on a line. Readers with a PRL above a scotoma tended to skip a line or reread the same line twice. In a follow-up study, seven subjects with a nonadvantageous PRL quickly developed a trained retinal locus (TRL) during instruction with an SLO. Although the readers developed the TRL in about 15 minutes, they read slower with the TRL than the PRL. This TRL research provides promising pilot data.
Content may be subject to copyright.
761
JRRD
JRRD
Volume 43, Number 6, Pages 761–770
September/October 2006
Journal of Rehabilitation Research & Development
Effects of preferred retinal locus placement on text navigation and
development of advantageous trained retinal locus
Gale R. Watson, MEd, CLVT;
1
*
Ronald A. Schuchard, PhD;
1
William R. De l’Aune, PhD;
2
Erica Watkins, BA
1
1
Blind Rehabilitation Service, Rehabilitation Research and Development Center of Excellence for Aging Veterans with
Vision Loss, Atlanta Department of Veterans Affairs Medical Center, Decatur, GA;
2
Atlanta Research and Education
Foundation, Atlanta, GA
Abstract—Sixty readers were evaluated for visual function and
text-navigation ability. The visual field and preferred retinal
locus (PRL) were measured with a scanning laser ophthalmo-
scope (SLO). We found significant differences in text-navigation
ability based on scotoma and PRL placement. Readers with a
PRL to the left of or above a scotoma had significantly less text-
navigation abilities. Readers with a PRL to the left of a scotoma
tended to misread words with similar beginnings and omit the
last word on a line. Readers with a PRL above a scotoma tended
to skip a line or reread the same line twice. In a follow-up study,
seven subjects with a nonadvantageous PRL quickly developed a
trained retinal locus (TRL) during instruction with an SLO.
Although the readers developed the TRL in about 15 minutes,
they read slower with the TRL than the PRL. This TRL research
provides promising pilot data.
Key words: low vision, macular scotoma, preferred retinal
locus, reading, rehabilitation, scanning laser ophthalmoscope,
text navigation, trained retinal locus, vision rehabilitation,
visual impairment.
INTRODUCTION
The term “preferred retinal locus” (PRL) describes a
retinal area that acts as a pseudofovea for visual tasks
when a central macular scotoma affects visual perform-
ance. Individuals with macular disease who are unable to
develop new skills with a PRL cannot optimally use low-
vision devices or effectively use their remaining vision
for reading [1]. The individual’s visual system may not
develop the best retinal area for reading with the PRL; for
example, when reading, part of the word may be placed
in the scotoma. Neither the individual nor the clinician
may be aware of this misplacement except possibly
through the clinician’s observation of reading errors
when the individual is reading aloud. Even individuals
with a paracentral scotoma may have a scotoma that is
next to the fovea and impairs reading. Low-vision reha-
bilitation effectively helps individuals with macular loss
regain their reading abilities.
Vision Function
The prevalence of persons with low vision and macular
scotomas is considerably larger than previously suspected.
Fletcher et al. found a macular scotoma prevalence of
Abbreviations: ETDRS = Early Treatment Diabetic Retinopa-
thy Study, logMAR = logarithm of minimum angle of resolu-
tion, MNREAD = Minnesota Low-Vision Reading (Acuity
Charts), OR = odds ratio, PRL = preferred retinal locus, SLO =
scanning laser ophthalmoscope, TRL = trained retinal locus,
VSRT = (Pepper) Visual Skills for Reading Test.
*
Address all correspondence to Gale R. Watson, MEd,
CLVT; Blind Rehabilitation Service, Rehabilitation
Research and Development Center of Excellence for Aging
Veterans with Vision Loss, Atlanta Department of Veter-
ans Affairs Medical Center, 151-R, 1670 Clairmont Road,
Decatur, GA 30033; 404-321-6111, ext 6789; fax: 404-728-
4837. Email: Gale.Watson@va.gov
DOI: 10.1682/JRRD.2005.07.0120
762
JRRD, Volume 43, Number 6, 2006
about 83 percent in their typical low-vision rehabilitation
service [2]. Less than half of these patients with low vision
were diagnosed by their referring ophthalmologist or low-
vision specialist as having macular scotomas.
Comprehensive rehabilitation can dramatically help
persons with visual impairments achieve full lifestyles
[3–4]. However, research has indicated that accurate, pre-
cise measurement of the scotoma area relative to the PRL
for visual tasks may be important, if not critical, for
visual performance tasks such as reading [5–9]. Macular
scotomas also impair visual functions such as contrast
sensitivity [10], contrast discrimination [11], stereo-
scopic depth perception [12], and fixation precision/sta-
bility [5,13].
Even when persons are aware of a scotoma (they see
a blurry, hazy gray area), they are seldom aware of the
true extent of the scotoma [14]. Persons typically
describe new scotomas resulting from laser treatments as
“large black spots” or “boulders,” but over a few months,
these spots fade and can no longer be localized without
perimetric testing. In an eye with a central scotoma
affecting the entire fovea, one or more eccentric PRLs
naturally and reliably develop [5–7,15–17]. A shift of the
oculocentric reference from the fovea to an eccentric
PRL is possible in persons with bilateral macular disease
[11,16].
Fletcher et al. used the scanning laser ophthalmo-
scope (SLO) to study the characteristics of macular
scotomas in 742 eyes [2]. The retinal locations of fixa-
tion, foveal PRL or eccentric PRL, were recorded and
graded. Dense scotomas were mapped with a 12 arc-
minute square target with a 50,000 Td retinal illumi-
nance. Relative scotomas were also mapped with the use
of targets at the retinal illuminance level that was deter-
mined to represent the threshold sensitivity of the fovea
or eccentric PRL. With this testing method, fixation
could be monitored. Even in persons with very unsteady
fixation, macular scotomas were identified in relationship
to the anatomy of the macula. The size, location, and
density of scotomas cannot be inferred consistently from
the ophthalmoscopic appearance of the macula. Figure 1
depicts an SLO retinal map.
In Fletcher et al.’s study [2], 83 percent (616/742) of
eyes had dense scotomatous areas present in the central
visual field, 14 percent (101/742) had scotomas of less
than 5° in diameter, and 69 percent (515/742) had scoto-
mas of greater than 5° in diameter. Scotoma shape varied
widely, from round scotomas centered on a nonfunction-
ing fovea to ring scotomas surrounding a functioning
fovea to highly complex amoeboid shapes. The PRL (for
fixation) was frequently near a dense scotoma. The rela-
tionship of the PRL to surrounding dense scotomas in the
visual field was categorized by how many of the four bor-
ders (superior, inferior, right, and left) around the PRL
were dense scotomas. (Note: a visual field dense scotoma
to the left of the PRL corresponds to an area of nonfunc-
tioning retina anatomically to the right of the PRL.) The
total number of dense-scotoma borders around the PRL
were (1) no borders: 22 percent, (2) one border: 46 per-
cent, (3) two borders: 14 percent, (4) three borders: 7 per-
cent, and (5) four borders (ring scotoma): 11 percent.
Fletcher and Schuchard have developed and used a
uniform PRL scoring system using an SLO to determine
measurable qualities of the PRL [18]. The SLO directly
determines the retinal location of visual stimuli with
respect to retinal image characteristics. Their retinal-
location scoring mechanism includes PRL size, fixation
ability, saccade ability, and pursuit ability. PRL size is
recorded in degrees. PRL fixation, saccade, and pursuit
abilities are scored from 0 to 4; 0 indicates no ability and
Figure 1.
Scanning laser ophthalmoscope (SLO) retinal map of scotoma shape
and size (outlined in green) and documented position and size o
f
nonfoveal preferred retinal locus (PRL) (outlined in red). Gree
n
outline of scotoma border is artistic rendering of dense scotoma (DS).
Green area encompasses nonseeing retinal points as designated b
y
manual hybrid perimetry with fixation monitoring in SLO. Note:
anatomic area of nonfunctioning retina anatomically to right of PRL
corresponds to visual field dense scotoma to left of PRL.
763
WATSON et al. Trained retinal locus for text navigation
4 indicates abilities that approximate those of a person
using foveal vision.
Reading with Preferred Retinal Locus
Programs that teach the visual skills used in reading
have been developed and widely used [19–21]. Previous
research has shown that training individuals with macular
degeneration in visual skills and use of low-vision
devices effectively increases reading accuracy [22–23],
rate [24–25], and comprehension [26–27].
Two oral reading assessments, the Pepper Visual
Skills for Reading Test (VSRT) and the Minnesota Low-
Vision Reading (MNREAD) Acuity Charts, have been
developed and standardized for persons with low vision
and are commonly used research measures. These assess-
ments were developed for quick evaluation of reading
ability, since persons with low vision are likely to read
more slowly and fatigue more quickly than persons with-
out visual impairments.
The Pepper VSRT is a highly reliable assessment of
text-navigation ability [28] and is used in low-vision clin-
ics for diagnosis and remediation of reading problems in
persons with macular degeneration [23,29]. The test can
be administered in approximately 10 minutes and can
diagnose specific reading errors of persons with macular
scotomas. Rate and accuracy scores and diagnoses of spe-
cific errors can be derived. This test was highly valid when
compared with the Gray Oral Reading Tests, a graded
reading evaluation of continuous text [30]. Classified error
codes were developed for providing an accuracy score.
Errors include omissions, insertions, misidentifications,
connecting words, separating words, and skipping lines.
The MNREAD Acuity Charts simply and quickly esti-
mate reading acuity, maximum reading rate, and critical
print size (smallest print size at which fluent reading can be
maintained) of persons with low vision [31]. Unlike the
Pepper VSRT, which evaluates visual skills in reading, the
MNREAD Acuity Charts assess reading acuity and rate in
a controlled manner and derive critical print size for most
fluent reading. The MNREAD Acuity Charts have high
test-retest reliability and are highly correlated with silent
reading ability (as measured by comprehension).
Fletcher et al. documented the characteristics of
macular scotomas relative to the PRL by relating SLO
macular perimetry to reading performance [1]. The PRL
ability (fixation stability, pursuit ability, and saccadic
ability) was rated with the 0 to 4 scoring system just
described [18]. This SLO test, a test of visual acuity
(Early Treatment Diabetic Retinopathy Study [ETDRS]
chart), and the MNREAD Acuity Charts [31] were
administered to 40 patients during the initial visit before
rehabilitation intervention. The MNREAD Acuity Charts
were administered as recommended. As in a previous
study [9], Snellen acuity accounted for only 19 percent of
the variability in reading rate, a scotoma above and/or
below the PRL accounted for only 12 percent, and fixa-
tion stability accounted for 27 percent. However, other
studied clinical variables had much stronger relationships
to reading rate than previously studied parameters. Sac-
cadic ability accounted for 38 percent of the variability in
reading rate, pursuit ability accounted for 37 percent, and
a scotoma to the right and/or left of the PRL accounted
for 40 percent. Subjects with scotomas to both the right
and left of the PRL or subjects with scotomas only to the
right had significantly reduced reading rates compared
with subjects with scotomas to the left or no scotomas on
either side of the PRL.
Trained Retinal Locus
Nilsson reported that, since the 1970s, a low-vision
service in Sweden has been instructing patients in the use
of a trained retinal locus (TRL) in a retinal area that is
“more advantageous to reading,” defined as above or
below an absolute central scotoma as opposed to the left
or right of the scotoma [25]. In Nilsson et al.’s feasibility
study, they trained a retinal locus in a scotomatous eye;
each subject’s other eye was unimpaired [32]. Their
results showed that subjects who did not develop a PRL
in the scotomatous eye (because they had one unimpaired
eye) developed a TRL in the scotomatous eye and had
significantly improved reading rates with the scotoma-
tous eye following training.
Nilsson et al. further reported on 20 persons with
age-related macular degeneration and central scotomas in
both eyes who were trained to develop a TRL [33]. In 18
subjects, the study eye was the lesser-seeing eye, since
the researchers wanted to avoid using subjects who had
already been trained to use eccentric viewing methods.
For the remaining two subjects, one had equal acuity in
both eyes and one had a rapidly progressing lesion that
prevented training of the lesser-seeing eye. None of their
subjects had been exposed to rehabilitation, had access to
magnification devices, or had been currently reading by
using their vision. Eleven subjects had a PRL to the left
of the lesion, six had a PRL to the upper-left of the lesion,
two had a PRL to the lower-left of the lesion, and one had
764
JRRD, Volume 43, Number 6, 2006
a PRL to the right of the lesion. The subjects were trained
via SLO. Subjects were taught to look at a letter in the
center of crossed lines; the letter was slowly moved so
that the subject saw the letter projected in a “trained reti-
nal locus” area, while the crossed lines remained in the
scotoma. The subject was told to continue looking at the
letter; the crossed lines remained as a reference point and
assured that the subject understood how to continue
viewing with the TRL.
Nilsson et al. reported that they could use the SLO to
constantly check the subject’s fixation and correct it if
the subject lost the fixation from the trained area [33].
After training with the SLO, subjects were taught to use
the TRL with high-plus magnifying spectacles [33].
Exercises with similar fixation lines were used [19].
Rather than use fixation and saccadic eye movements, the
subjects were taught to scroll printed reading exercises so
that the words moved into the TRL field of view.
The authors reported that 18 of the 20 subjects devel-
oped a TRL [33]. Of the remaining two subjects, one was
unable to use a TRL after 5 hours of training and one
refused to use the magnifying spectacles. Of the 18 sub-
jects who developed a TRL, 12 were trained to use a TRL
below the visual-field scotoma and 6 were taught to use a
TRL above the visual-field scotoma. Subjects received an
average of 5.4 hours of training, with 1 week of home-
work between each training session. Mean reading rate
prior to training was 9.7 words/minute; after training,
mean reading rate was 68.3 words/minute. The authors
concluded that their subjects were able to be trained to
use a TRL and following rehabilitation, study eyes
showed significantly increased reading rates with the
TRL. However, the authors did not train the subjects’
naturally occurring PRL, so we do not know what ability
the study eyes might have acquired had their PRL been
trained. In earlier studies [25], these researchers achieved
dramatic and similar increases in reading rates (from 0 to
75.5 words/minute) through training for approximately
the same rehabilitation time period but without direct
monitoring of eye movements. One must question, then,
whether training is the most important aspect of reading
rate improvement, regardless of whether the reader uses a
PRL or TRL.
We do not know what ability Nilsson et al.’s subjects
might have gained if their better-seeing eyes had been
trained. The better-seeing eye is generally chosen for
rehabilitation in low-vision clinics unless a patient can
read with a binocular low-vision device, such as a closed-
circuit television system or a hand/stand magnifier.
Nilsson et al. also concluded that a PRL to the left or
right of a scotoma was not advantageous and a PRL
above or below a scotoma was better for maximal read-
ing ability [33]. Unfortunately, these conclusions are
based on clinical heuristics and laboratory research on
subjects without visual impairments who were provided
with artificial scotomas [34–35]. In a study of readers
with low vision with naturally occurring central scoto-
mas, Fletcher et al. did not find an advantage of any par-
ticular PRL-scotoma position for reading rate [1].
Our study investigated whether PRL and scotoma
placement were related to text-navigation ability in sub-
jects with low vision. We also investigated whether a
TRL could be developed in the better-seeing eyes of sub-
jects with low vision who were long-term PRL users.
EXPERIMENT 1
Methods
We recruited 60 subjects who had visual impairments
due to macular diseases (absolute scotoma in the visual
field as measured by the SLO perimetry), 20/800 or bet-
ter acuity in the better-seeing eye, and interest in reading
with low-vision devices. We obtained the subjects’
informed consent using the procedures required by the
Emory University Human Investigations Committee.
Following informed consent, subjects were evaluated
without low-vision devices by the following procedures:
1. ETDRS Distance Visual Acuity Charts: Distance acuity.
2. MNREAD Acuity Charts: Reading acuity, reading
rate, and critical print size.
3. Pepper VSRT (with low-vision devices): Text naviga-
tion ability, reading accuracy, and reading rate.
4. SLO testing: Macular perimetry as described by Sun-
ness et al. [36] and PRL characteristics, including fixa-
tion stability, as described by Fletcher and Schuchard
[18]. The confocal SLO with graphics capabilities
allows an examiner to directly determine the retinal
location of visual stimuli on the retinal image in real
time. The SLO with graphics capabilities continuously
obtains retinal images with a nearly invisible infrared
laser (780 nm) and scans graphics (e.g., lines, letters,
and words) on the retina with a modulated visible red-
light laser (633 nm). Thus, the subject observes the
stimuli, which the examiner sees directly on the
765
WATSON et al. Trained retinal locus for text navigation
subject’s retina. The retinal illuminance of the stimuli is
adjustable by 256 steps within the visible red-light laser
range (50–50,000 Td). The SLO provides a 32° × 22°
image of the retina with a minimum resolution of
approximately 4 arcminutes for measurement of the reti-
nal areas and positioning of targets. Macular scotomas
can be mapped on the SLO with kinetic, static, or hybrid
testing techniques.
5. Binocular perception test: Evaluation of dominant
PRL. A computer system displayed stereo pairs
sequentially on a monitor at 120 Hz. The electronic
eyewear, liquid crystal shutters synchronized with the
monitor, determined which eye saw each frame of the
stereo pair. When the left image was on the video
screen, for example, the left shutter opened while the
right shutter closed. The subject was told to fixate a 1°
star (seen as a cross by the right eye, an X by the left
eye, and a star binocularly), then report which target
was seen. If the subject reported a cross or an X, then
he or she was not seeing the visual information from
the PRL/fovea of both eyes during binocular viewing.
Results
Table 1 shows subjects’ ages; scores on vision func-
tion, reading, and visual acuity measures; and PRL per-
formance.
We determined fixation size for the PRL by measur-
ing fixation eye movements for 20 seconds; the diameter
that we recorded encompassed the area of fixation eye
movements [18]. We reported PRL performance using
the previously described method of rating from 0 to 4 the
subject’s use of the PRL in the SLO for fixation, pursuit,
and saccade abilities [18]; 0 indicated no ability and
4 indicated that the subject’s off-foveal PRL ability was
similar to that for a foveal PRL.
Rate, accuracy, and error scores on the Pepper VSRT
were recorded and matched to the PRL recorded on the
SLO. We coded text-navigation errors on the Pepper
VSRT as left directional errors, right directional errors,
or below directional errors. Left directional errors were
missing or confusing the beginning of a word or missing
the first word on a line. Right directional errors were
missing or confusing the ending of a word or missing the
last word on a line. Below directional errors were skip-
ping a line or reading the same line twice. In this article,
we report results for text-navigation errors only; other
results will be published later.
We scored scotomas by placing a matrix over the
SLO field plot. The matrix was a transparency with con-
centric circles, a center fixation dot, and eight radial lines
from the center fixation dot. The fixation dot was cen-
tered over the PRL; each of the radials was examined. By
using this overlay, we determined the presence or
absence of a dense scotoma on each radial. Scotoma con-
figuration may have included several radials. Figure 2
depictions the matrix transparency over an SLO map.
For this analysis, we used dense scotomas in the eye
with the dominant PRL; if no dominant PRL existed, we
used dense scotomas in the subject’s preferred eye.
Table 1.
Ages and scores on vision function and reading measures for
60 subjects with absolute scotoma in visual field in Experiment 1.
Measure Mean ± SD Range
Age (yr) 74.5 ± 10.6
23
Acuity (logMAR) 0.8 ± 0.3 0.1–1.5
PRL Size (° fixation) 3.9 ± 2.1 1.5–10
Fixation Ability Score 3.0 ± 0.7 0–4
Pursuit Score 2.5 ± 0.7 0–4
Saccade Score 2.3 ± 0.9 0–4
MNREAD Critical Print Size (logMAR) 1.1 ± 0.4 0.2–1.6
MNREAD Acuity (logMAR) 0.9 ± 0.4 0.1–1.7
MNREAD Rate (words/min) 78.1 ± 52.4 4–210
Pepper VSRT Rate (words/min) 29.0 ± 17.6 3.5–81.8
Pepper VSRT Accuracy (%) 83.8 ± 19.4 20.8–99.3
logMAR = logarithm of minimum angle of resolution, MNREAD = Minnesota
Low-Vision Reading (Acuity Charts), PRL = preferred retinal locus, SD =
standard deviation, VSRT = Visual Skills for Reading Test.
Figure 2.
Transparency overlay for scoring scotomas. Matrix was transparency
with concentric circles, center fixation dot (outlined in red), and eight
radial lines from center fixation dot. Scotoma is outlined in green.
766
JRRD, Volume 43, Number 6, 2006
We coded subjects’ reading errors on the Pepper
VSRT as specific directional errors; “right” errors were
mistaking the ending of a word or omitting the last word
on a line, “left” errors were mistaking the beginning of a
word or omitting the first word on a line, and “below”
errors were skipping or rereading a line.
We performed a simple analysis of the presence or
absence of directional reading errors (right, left, below)
from the Pepper VSRT and the presence or absence of
scotomas in each radial from the PRL. In each case, we
constructed a fourfold table and performed a chi-square
analysis. A total of 24 fourfold tables were constructed
and analyzed. For each table, we evaluated the signifi-
cance of the resultant chi-square
value, and if it was sta-
tistically significant, we computed and reported an odds
ratio (OR). We obtained the following results:
A dense scotoma to the right of the PRL (radial 3)
was associated with more right errors (
χ
2
= 4.1, p =
0.05). The OR indicated that a scotoma in this radial
was associated with 2.033 times the likelihood of
right errors. A dense scotoma to the left of the PRL
(radial 7) was associated with one-third fewer right
errors (OR = 0.348;
χ
2
= 3.9, p = 0.048).
Left errors were not associated with any particular
PRL configuration.
A dense scotoma below the PRL (radial 1) was asso-
ciated with below errors (
χ
2
= 4.2, p = 0.04). The OR
indicated that a scotoma in this radial was associated
with 3.368 times the likelihood of below errors. A
dense scotoma below and to the right of the PRL
(radial 2) was also associated with below errors (
χ
2
=
5.9, p = 0.015). The OR indicated that a scotoma on
this radial was associated with 4.474 times the likeli-
hood of below errors.
EXPERIMENT 2
Methods
In the second experiment, we recruited subjects
whose naturally occurring PRL was to the left of their
scotoma and trained them to develop a TRL that was
below their scotoma. We chose the PRL location based
on our and others’ research that found this location to be
disadvantageous for reading, and we chose the TRL loca-
tion based on our and others’ research that found this
location to be advantageous for reading. To locate sub-
jects whose PRL was to the left of their scotoma, we
screened the records of the 60 subjects from Experiment
1 for whom we already had documented SLO visual
fields. Of these 60 subjects, 13 fit our criterion but 3 had
either moved or were ill. The remaining 10 potential sub-
jects provided informed consent according to the require-
ments of the Emory University Human Investigations
Committee and were pretested for acuity and central
visual-field perimetry via SLO. Three subjects had
changed PRL locations (two had continued macular loss
and one deliberately changed his PRL, as explained in
the “Discussion” section). This resulted in seven subjects
who agreed to participate. Because of the very small sam-
ple size, this experiment was exploratory.
We administered the retinal locus characteristics and
ability scales using the SLO [18], the Pepper VSRT
[28,30], and the MNREAD Acuity Charts [31]. We
instructed subjects in development of a TRL below the
scotoma and in use of this TRL for reading words pre-
sented on the SLO. Following this instruction, we read-
ministered all tests.
Results
We could not completely ascertain whether subjects
were reading with the PRL or TRL when they were view-
ing the reading charts in posttesting; however, subjects
verbalized their understanding and monitored their own
eye movements after training. They demonstrated appro-
priate eccentric viewing techniques when asked to view
the experimenters face and other near targets, showing
that they understood and could use the TRL, rather than
the PRL, in posttesting.
The average age of the subjects was 75.7, with a range
from 68 to 82. The average subject acuity was right eye =
1.0 logarithm of minimum angle of resolution (logMAR)
(range 0.64–1.26), left eye = 0.98 logMAR (range 0.48–
1.52), both eyes = 0.91 logMAR (range 0.64–1.12), and
dominant PRL = 0.95 logMAR (range 0.64–1.18).
In our SLO protocol, we used a fixation target (cross)
in the PRL as a “tag” and another target (letter) for sec-
ondary fixation with the TRL. We gave the subject verbal
feedback while viewing his or her performance in the
SLO to assist development of the TRL. The subject was
told to “look at” the cross in the PRL and “notice” the let-
ter in the TRL. We used this method to help the subject
understand the difference between the PRL and TRL and
avoid a situation in which the subject was tempted in the
early training stage to “look” with the PRL at the TRL
target. Targets in the TRL were initially single letters and
767
WATSON et al. Trained retinal locus for text navigation
then two-letter words, three-letter words, etc. Figure 3
depicts the SLO map during TRL training.
When the subject consistently fixated, used the TRL
for eye movements, and read several successive words
with the TRL, we extinguished the PRL fixation target
(cross). The TRL was subsequently used for further eye
movement and reading instruction. We gave the subjects
a short break, then had them read again in the SLO. All
subjects were able to read with the TRL after the break;
none reverted to the PRL to read words in the SLO.
Table 2 provides the subjects’ PRL and TRL charac-
teristics. All subjects developed a TRL after an approxi-
mately 15-minute training session. This training session
with the SLO resulted in a TRL that closely matched the
PRL in size and in fixation, saccade, and pursuit abilities.
The TRL and PRL abilities did not significantly differ.
Subjects read significantly slower and with less text-
navigation ability with the TRL than the PRL. However,
considering that the training session was short, provided
exclusively in the SLO, and did not include practice fol-
lowing the SLO intervention, subject performance was
surprisingly responsive.
DISCUSSION
Experiment 1 showed very strong associations
between scotoma placement and reading errors on the
Pepper VSRT. We found that a scotoma to the right of the
PRL was related to reading errors on the right (missing
the end of words or the last word on the line of print). A
scotoma below the PRL was strongly related to reading
performance (both errors of skipping or rereading a line
of print and slower reading rate), a finding that has never
been reported in the literature. We expected to find that
scotoma to the left of the PRL would be related to reading
errors on the left (missing or confusing the beginning of a
Figure 3.
Scanning laser ophthalmoscope (SLO) image during trained retinal
locus (TRL) instruction of subject with central dense scotoma (DS)
(outlined in green). Subject looked at cross (+) with preferred retinal
locus (PRL) (outlined in red). Subject was asked to look at + and
notice V shown in TRL. V and + are superimposed on SLO image.
Once subject understood and complied with TRL training, + was
extinguished. SLO image vertically inverted as would be seen by
experimenter. Subject would see V below scotoma, while
experimenter would see V above scotoma.
Table 2.
Preferred retinal locus (PRL) and trained retinal locus (TRL) characteristics for seven subjects in Experiment 2.
Characteristic
PRL TRL
Mean Minimum Maximum Mean Minimum Maximum
Fixation Ability Score 3 3 3 3 3 3
PRL Size (° diameter) 5.1 3.5 6.0 5.1 4.0 7.0
Saccade Ability Score 2.4 2.0 3.0 2.0 1.0 3.0
Pursuit Ability Score 3.0 3.0 3.0 2.2 1.0 3.0
Reading Ability
MNREAD Rate (words/min) 74.4 19 135 34.1 8 92
MNREAD Acuity (logMAR) 1.14 0.71 1.42 1.34 1.02 1.66
Critical Print Size (logMAR) 1.33 0.80 1.60 1.45 1.30 1.60
Pepper VSRT Rate (words/min) 27.1 12.3 41.2 17.1 11.3 29.6
Pepper VSRT Accuracy (%) 94.4 87.9 99.3 72.6 54.6 94.7
logMAR = logarithm of minimum angle of resolution, MNREAD = Minnesota Low Vision Reading (Acuity Charts), VSRT = Visual Skills for Reading Test.
768
JRRD, Volume 43, Number 6, 2006
word or missing the first word on a line), but this proved
not to be the case.
Other low-vision clinicians and clinical researchers
have suggested that a PRL above or below a scotoma is
the most suitable PRL for reading because the scotoma
does not interfere with scanning across the line of print
[19,25,33,37]. Our data show that a PRL above the
scotoma is strongly associated with rereading lines or
skipping lines, which severely disrupts the reading pro-
cess. Our findings of the deleterious effects of a PRL
above a central scotoma make intuitive sense. Because
we read English from left to right and top to bottom, a
scotoma in this position always obscures that which is
below fixation. Because most languages are read from
the top of a page to the bottom, even non-English readers
with such a scotoma may have this difficulty.
Other low-vision clinicians and researchers have also
stated that a PRL to the right of a scotoma is a disadvan-
tageous reading position because a scotoma in this posi-
tion would ostensibly cause errors in scanning a line of
print [19,25,33,37]. Although problematic in the labora-
tory for readers without visual impairments using artifi-
cial scotomas, our research does not show that readers
with macular scotomas are disadvantaged. Our readers
with scotomas in this position actually showed an advan-
tage in reading the ends of words and the last word on a
line correctly. A PRL to the right of the scotoma may
provide suitable reading ability.
The results of Experiment 1 challenge former think-
ing about the relationship between scotoma placement and
reading performance. Our data suggest that the position of
the PRL relative to the scotoma may be important for
readers with macular loss but not as important as was pre-
viously thought. Prevailing clinical heuristics that a
scotoma to the right of the PRL is deleterious and a
scotoma above the PRL is advantageous appear to have
been validated. However, a scotoma below the PRL may
impair performance more than previously thought and a
scotoma to the left of the PRL may be more benign. In
clinics providing PRL training, low-vision clinicians can
use this information to better understand their patients’
reading performance.
Another result of our research is that error scores on
the Pepper VSRT are useful for determining the effect of
PRL placement on text navigation. The error scores as
coded and used in this research may be a quick, expedi-
ent proxy for determining PRL position in the absence of
an SLO. A fixation target in a fundus camera [32] or
visuoscope for determining retinal fixation position can
provide this information as well and has been used suc-
cessfully as an SLO proxy.
Although only a small number of subjects who were
screened qualified for Experiment 2, all subjects in this
experiment were able to develop a TRL in their better-
seeing eye with a well-developed PRL after approxi-
mately 15 minutes of SLO training. This SLO training
session resulted in a TRL that closely matched the PRL in
size and in fixation, saccade, and pursuit abilities. We did
not measure the distance of the PRL and TRL from the
fovea, so we do not know whether fixation stability was
similar because of this reason. The fixation scoring scale
may be a relatively coarse measure, and more precise
measures might have shown more differences in fixation
stability, pursuits, and saccades.
Subjects read significantly slower and with less text-
navigation ability with the TRL than the PRL. However,
considering that the training session was very short, pro-
vided exclusively in the SLO environment, and did not
include practice following the SLO intervention, subject
performance was surprisingly good. These subjects were
regular readers with a longstanding PRL; therefore, their
uniform ability to develop and use a TRL in the better-
seeing eye after a 15-minute SLO training intervention is
very encouraging. The results, while suggestive, were not
subjected to inferential statistical testing and must be
interpreted in terms of their exploratory nature. However,
we conclude, as do other researchers, that TRL develop-
ment is an area that deserves more research and clinical
attention.
Our subjects had long-standing macular loss (dura-
tion of at least 1 year) and were experienced low-vision
device users. They had been subjects in various experi-
ments at our center and volunteered eagerly for various
reasons: to gain more information about their visual
impairments, to enjoy a novel experience, to understand
whether something new might assist them in daily life,
etc. Some personality mechanism or other psychosocial
characteristic may have motivated these low-vision read-
ers to become research subjects and therefore respond
willingly and easily to our TRL training.
On the other hand, this is the first report of training a
TRL in the better-seeing eye of patients with macular loss
who were using their better-seeing eye with low-vision
devices for reading. These readers developed a TRL with
relative ease and speed. Whether it is possible for the gen-
eral clinical population in a low vision service to do so
769
WATSON et al. Trained retinal locus for text navigation
and find the TRL as useful as a PRL remains to be seen.
We do not know how long the effect of this short-term
TRL development lasted because we did not follow the
subjects.
One subject screened for Experiment 2 reported that
he had deliberately taught himself to use a TRL. Whereas
he had earlier demonstrated a PRL to the left of the
scotoma, at the recall visit, he demonstrated a retinal locus
below the scotoma. When the experimenter remarked on
this change, the subject stated that he had requested and
received his Pepper VSRT results from Experiment 1,
which demonstrated right directional errors. Upon return-
ing home, he taught himself to change his fixation and
found that reading was easier. Although he was dismissed
as a subject, the results of his enterprising self-training and
our study are in agreement and are encouraging.
CONCLUSIONS
Contrary to clinical heuristics, we found that the text-
navigation performance of our readers with macular
degeneration who had a PRL to the right of a scotoma did
not suffer unduly. We further found that text-navigation
of readers with a PRL above a scotoma was more prob-
lematic than previously believed. This research questions
some of the commonly used assessment and training
techniques developed for this population. Although clini-
cal heuristics are prevalent in medicine and rehabilitation
as quick “rules-of-thumb,” for practice, they must be
carefully evaluated.
Our subset of readers was able to quickly and reliably
develop a TRL in a position that was advantageous for
scanning text. Promising lines of future research are to
discover whether other advantageous TRL positions
(such as to the right of the scotoma) exist, whether a
longer training and practice session would improve per-
formance, and whether persons with macular loss can be
taught this technique without use of an SLO.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
This material was based on work supported by the
Department of Veterans Affairs, Rehabilitation Research
and Development Service (VA RR&D), grant C849-RA
(Gale R. Watson); and the Atlanta VA RR&D Center of
Excellence for Aging Veterans with Vision Loss (Gale R.
Watson).
The authors have declared that no competing inter-
ests exist.
REFERENCES
1. Fletcher DC, Schuchard RA, Watson GR. Relative loca-
tions of macular scotomas near the PRL: Effect on low
vision reading. J Rehabil Res Dev. 1999;36(4):56–64.
[PMID: 10678458]
2. Fletcher DC, Schuchard RA, Livingstone CL, Crane WG,
Hu SY. Scanning laser ophthalmoscope (SLO) macular
perimetry and applications for low vision rehabilitation cli-
nicians. Ophthalmol Clin N Am. 1994;7:257–65.
3. Maino JH, Carty RE. Part II. VICTORS: A model for the
provision of low vision services to the partially sighted vet-
eran. J Am Optom Assoc. 1983;54(11):991–93.
[PMID: 6643902]
4. Watson GR, De l’Aune WR, Stelmack JA, Maino JH, Long
S. National survey of the impact of low vision device use
among veterans. Optom Vis Sci. 1997;74(5):249–59.
[PMID: 9219282]
5. Timberlake GT, Mainster MA, Peli E, Augliere RA,
Essock EA, Arend LE. Reading with a macular scotoma. I.
Retinal location of scotoma and fixation area. Invest Oph-
thalmol Vis Sci. 1986;27(7):1137–47. [PMID: 3721792]
6. Timberlake GT, Peli E, Essock EA, Augliere RA. Reading
with a macular scotoma. II. Retinal locus for scanning text.
Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1987;28(8):1268–74.
[PMID: 3610545]
7. Cummings RW, Whittaker SG, Watson GR, Budd JM.
Scanning characters and reading with a central scotoma.
Am J Optom Physiol Opt. 1985;62(12):833–43.
[PMID: 4083327]
8. Legge GE, Rubin GS, Pelli DG, Schleske MM. Psycho-
physics of reading—II. Low vision. Vision Res. 1985;
25(2):253–66. [PMID: 4013092]
9. Legge GE, Ross JA, Isenberg LM, LaMay JM. Psycho-
physics of reading. Clinical predictors of low-vision read-
ing speed. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1992;33(3):677–87.
[PMID: 1544792]
10. Mitra S. Spatial contrast sensitivity in macular disorder.
Doc Ophthalmol. 1985;59(3):247–67. [PMID: 4006670]
11. Schuchard RA, Raasch TW. Retinal locus for fixation:
Pericentral fixation targets. Clin Vis Sci. 1992;7:511–620.
12. Raasch TW. Method for assessing stereopsis and positional
sensitivity in normally-sighted and low vision observers.
Opt Soc Am Tech Dig Ser. 1991;1:109–12.
770
JRRD, Volume 43, Number 6, 2006
13. Schuchard RA, Fletcher DC, Maino JH. A scanning laser
ophthalmoscope low vision rehabilitation system. Eye Clin
Vis Care. 1994;6:101–7.
14. Schuchard RA. Validity and interpretation of Amsler grid
reports. Arch Ophthalmol. 1993;111(6):776–80.
[PMID: 8512478]
15. Whittaker SG, Budd JM, Cummings RW. Eccentric fixa-
tion with macular scotoma. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci.
1988;29(2):268–78. [PMID: 3338884]
16. White JM, Bedell HE. The oculomotor reference in
humans with bilateral macular disease. Invest Ophthalmol
Vis Sci. 1990;31(6):1149–61. [PMID: 2354915]
17. Schuchard RA, Fletcher DC. Preferred retinal locus. A review
with applications in low vision rehabilitation. Ophthalmol
Clin N Am. 1994;7:243–56.
18. Fletcher DC, Schuchard RA. Preferred retinal loci relation-
ship to macular scotomas in a low-vision population. Oph-
thalmology. 1997;104(4):632–38. [PMID: 9111255]
19. Bäckman O, Inde K. Low vision training. Malmö (Swe-
den): LiberHermods; 1979.
20. Watson GR, Berg RV. Near training techniques. In: Jose
RT, editor. Understanding low vision. New York (NY):
American Foundation for the Blind; 1983. p. 317–62.
21. Wright VW, Watson GR. Learn to use your vision for read-
ing workbook. Lilburn (GA): Bear Consultants; 1995.
22. Leat SJ, Fryer A, Rumney NJ. Outcome of low vision aid
provision: The effectiveness of a low vision clinic. Optom
Vis Sci. 1994;71(3):199–206. [PMID: 8196946]
23. Stelmack JA, Stelmack TR, Fraim M, Warrington J. Clini-
cal uses of the Pepper Visual Skills for Reading Test in low
vision rehabilitation. Am J Optom Physiol Opt. 1987;
64(11):829–31. [PMID: 3425678]
24. Goodrich GL, Mehr EB, Darling NC. Parameters in the use
of CCTV’s and optical aids. Am J Optom Physiol Opt.
1980;57(12):881–92. [PMID: 6164297]
25. Nilsson U. Visual rehabilitation with and without educa-
tional training in the use of optical aids and residual vision.
A Prospective study of patients with age-related macular
degeneration. Clin Vis Sci. 1990;6:3–10.
26. Fridal G, Jansen L, Klindt M. Courses in reading develop-
ment for partially sighted students. J Vis Impair Blind. 1981;
75(1):4–7.
27. Watson GR, Wright V, De l’Aune WR. The efficacy of
comprehension training and reading practice for print read-
ers with macular loss. J Vis Impair Blind. 1992;86(1):37–43.
28. Baldasare J, Watson GR, Whittaker SG, Miller-Schafer H.
The development and evaluation of a reading test for low
vision individuals with macular loss. J Vis Impair Blind.
1986;80(6):785–89.
29. Stelmack JA, Reda D, Ahlers S, Bainbridge L, McCray J.
Reading performance of geriatric patients post exudative
maculopathy. J Am Optom Assoc. 1991;62(1):53–57.
[PMID: 1813493]
30. Watson GR, Baldasare J, Whittaker SG. The validity and
clinical uses of the Pepper Visual Skills for Reading Test.
J Vis Impair Blind. 1990;84(3):119–23.
31. Legge GE, Ross JA, Luebker A, LaMay JM. Psychophysics
of reading. VIII. The Minnesota Low-Vision Reading Test.
Optom Vis Sci. 1989;66(12):843–53. [PMID: 2626251]
32. Nilsson UL, Frennesson C, Nilsson SE. Location and stability
of a newly established eccentric retinal locus suitable for
reading, achieved through training of patients with a dense
central scotoma. Optom Vis Sci. l998;75(12):873–78.
[PMID: 9875992]
33. Nilsson UL, Frennesson C, Nilsson SE. Patients with AMD
and a large absolute central scotoma can be trained suc-
cessfully to use eccentric viewing, as demonstrated in a
scanning laser ophthalmoscope. Vision Res. 2003;43(16):
1777–87. [PMID: 12818347]
34. Cummings RW, Rubin GS. Reading speed and saccadic eye
movements with an artificial paracentral scotoma [abstract].
Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1992;33(Suppl):1418.
35. Fine EM. Reading with central scotoma: What can we
learn from simulation studies? Vis Impair Res. 1999;1(3):
165–73.
36. Sunness JS, Schuchard RA, Shen N, Rubin GS, Dagnelie
G, Haselwood DM. Landmark-driven fundus perimetry
using the scanning laser ophthalmoscope. Invest Ophthal-
mol Vis Sci. 1995;36(9):1863–74. [PMID: 7635660]
37. McMahon TT, Hansen M, Stelmack JA, Oliver P, Viana
MA. Saccadic eye movements as a measure of the effect of
low vision rehabilitation on reading rate. Optom Vis Sci.
1993;70(6):506–10. [PMID: 8336914]
Submitted for publication July 7, 2005. Accepted in
revised form February 27, 2006.
... respectively. After training, the values increased to 32 The intersession variation of fixation parameters P1 and P2 values showed a consistent rise in each session. Statistical analysis of intersessional change showed statistically significant increases after the fifth session compared to the pre-training value (p<0.001). ...
... 31 Nilsson et al. 8 reported initial outcomes for TRL training and found improved reading rates in scotomatous eyes following 5.4 hours of training with scanning laser ophthalmoscope. Watson et al. 32 trained the better-seeing eye and reported that the development of a TRL was easy and fast. In contrast, Baker et al. 4 observed that eyes with more severe foveal scarring were more prone to reorganization. ...
Article
Full-text available
Objectives: This study evaluated acoustic biofeedback training using microperimetry in patients with foveal scars and an eligible retinal locus for better fixation. Materials and methods: A total of 29 eligible patients were enrolled in the study. The acoustic biofeedback training module in the MAIA (Macular Integrity Assessment, CenterVue®, Italy) microperimeter was used for training. To determine the treatment efficacy, the following variables were compared before and after testing: best corrected visual acuity (BCVA); MAIA microperimeter full threshold 4-2 test parameters of average threshold value, fixation parameters P1 and P2, and bivariate contour ellipse area (BCEA) for 63% and 95% of fixation points; contrast sensitivity (CSV 1000E Contrast Sensitivity Test); reading speed using the Minnesota Low-Vision Reading Test (MNREAD reading chart); and quality of life (NEI-VFQ-25). In addition, fixation stability parameters were recorded during each session. Results: The study group consisted of 29 patients with a mean age of 68.72±8.34 years. Median BCVA was initially 0.8 (0.2-1.6) logMAR and was 0.8 (0.1-1.6) logMAR after 8 weeks of preferred retinal locus training (p=0.003). The fixation stability parameter P1 improved from a mean of 21.28±3.08% to 32.69±3.69% (p=0.001) while mean P2 improved from 52.79±4.53% to 68.31±3.89% (p=0.001). Mean BCEA 63% decreased from 16.11±2.27°2 to 13.34±2.26°2 (p=0.127) and mean BCEA 95% decreased from 45.87±6.72°2 to 40.01±6.78°2 (p=0.247) after training. Binocular reading speed was 38.28±6.25 words per minute (wpm) before training and 45.34±7.35 wpm after training (p<0.001). Statistically significant improvement was observed in contrast sensitivity and quality of life questionnaire scores after training. Conclusion: Beginning with the fifth session, biofeedback training for a new trained retinal locus improved average sensitivity, fixation stability, reading speed, contrast sensitivity, and quality of life in patients with macular scarring.
... A limited area of horizontal field is used for left-toright reading and fixation and scotoma characteristics will determine the horizontal span for reading. 75,77 Scotoma patterns that limit the horizontal span for reading may limit both reading fluency and the ability to use magnification. One clinical example of this is patients with macular degeneration who have a scotoma pattern that encircles the fovea, leaving a limited horizontal span for reading or using magnification (foveal-sparing scotoma pattern). ...
... 32 Furthermore, scotomas at the right of fixation hide the terminal of words, which may be easier to predict, compared to left scotomas hiding the beginning, though it is shown that saccadic movements are more helpful in left scotomas. 21,33 Scotomas below fixation or preferred retinal loci (PRL) are fairly disabled and lead to the omission of whole lines of text. We should take into account the development of PRL in many patients and their location concerning scotoma. ...
Article
Full-text available
Purpose To present a novel smartphone-based application (GDRS-test, Greek Digital Reading Speed - test) for the assessment of reading speed, to evaluate, whether this test could be easily and reliably used by patients with visual impairment and normal individuals serving as an adjunctive tool for their visual examination. Patients and Methods One hundred and five visually impaired and 32 normal eyes were examined. Depending on existing active ocular pathology, patients were divided into a non-macular and a macular group, We examined the reading performance for continuous text (MNREAD chart) and random unrelated words of the new smartphone-based reading speed app, monocularly. The patients’ best-corrected visual acuity ranged from 1.3 to 0.2 logMAR. Examinees were asked to read aloud (at a 40 cm distance) a series of 30 random two-syllable and then 30 three-syllable Greek words, without semantic connection. Reading speed was measured as correct words per minute for critical print size. The individuals were examined twice within 2 weeks for test-retest reliability. Correlations and comparisons concerning each group adjusted for age and visual acuity were performed. Results There was moderate correlation between MNREAD and 2SYL SPEED (Reading speed for 2-syllable) (Pearson's rho = 0.589, p < 0.001) and 3SYL SPEED (Reading speed for 3-syllable) (Pearson's rho = 0.617, p < 0.001) for healthy individuals. The mean 2SYL SPEED and 3SYL SPEED for Individuals of the maculopathies group or non-maculopathies group were significantly lower compared to normal individuals adjusted for age and visual acuity [B (95% CI): −93.077 (−104.165, −81.98), p < 0.001] and [B (95% CI): −92.254 (−104.196, −80.312), p < 0.001], respectively. The test-retest analysis showed a good agreement for patients and healthy individuals. Conclusion The novel-reading speed application for the Greek-speaking population was found to accurately detect differences between patients with visual impairment and healthy individuals. It was designed and constructed with the intention to ease and improve the ophthalmic examination allowing individuals to self-evaluate reading speed by transmitting the result to their physician.
... 23 Currently, eccentric viewing training is used in the rehabilitation of such patients. 19,[24][25][26][27][28][29][30][31] Therefore, it is very important to select an appropriate area on the retina for training. Since there is not any research on reading indices in Persian-speaking patients, as a consequence, the findings of this study may be used to select an appropriate location in the retina for training. ...
... It can be said that reading is significantly slower in patients who have not been trained in the use of the retinal locus, but, according to the findings of Watson et al., this does not mean that PRL training should not be further investigated [43]. ...
Chapter
Full-text available
Age-related macular degeneration (AMD) is a common, chronic, and progressive eye disease that is considered the leading cause of visual loss among the elderly in developed countries. Advanced AMD, including choroidal neovascularization (CNV) or geographic atrophy (GA), is associated with substantial and progressive visual impairment that can lead to a significant reduction in functional independence and quality of life (QoL) for affected individuals, whose number is expected to increase in the coming years in line with population growth and ageing. In this context, while an important part of medical care is focused on preventing the progression of the disease, Visual Rehabilitation (VR) aims to address its consequences by providing these patients with a number of strategies to achieve their goals and participate autonomously, actively and productively in society. This chapter aims to provide an update on evidence-based practices in the field and how modern technologies play an important role in the development of new VR approaches.
... Previous studies demonstrated that reading ability and speed are worse in patients with central deep scotoma and loss of foveal fixation than in healthy subjects. 6,14,22,36,[38][39][40][41][42][43][44][45][46] In binocular testing, the highest correlation (though statistical significance was not achieved) was obtained between saccade duration and contrast sensitivity (LogCS: Pelli Robson, REX test). This result, if confirmed in a larger sample of patients, would suggest that a lower saccade speed could be due to decreased retinal contrast sensitivity in the PRL area. ...
Article
Purpose: To investigate saccadic movements in subjects with eccentric fixation due to a deep central scotoma in Stargardt disease (STGD). Methods: We studied 10 patients with STGD and 10 healthy subjects (control group). Saccadic movements of all the 20 subjects were assessed by using the eye tracker technique Tobii Glasses Pro 2. Standard measurements of reading ability (MNREAD charts), visual acuity (ETDRS charts), contrast sensitivity (Pelli-Robson charts), reading contrast threshold and speed (REX charts), retinal sensitivity and stability and localization of the fixation (MP1 fundus perimetry) were obtained in all subjects. Results: The saccadic movements time was significantly slower in STGD than in healthy subjects (699 ± 193 ms vs 299 ± 40 ms, p < 0.001). When STGD patients moved fixation to the target localized in retinal scotomatous areas, the movement was significantly slower compared to non scotomatous areas in the retina (1103 ± 798 ms vs 524 ± 187 ms, p = 0.039). There was a trend toward a correlation between slow saccadic movements in STGD subjects and the reading performance indices, although statistical significance was not achieved. Conclusion: Ocular saccades guided by eccentric fixation in STGD patients are significantly slower than in the control group, especially when the target corresponds to retinal areas with a deep scotoma. These results can explain the worse reading performance in STGD subjects, in particular when a non-viewing area on the right part of the text is present.
... 120 However, for people with AMD, studies have shown that reading rate is not strongly related to the position of the PRL, 121,122 although Watson et al. show that reading errors were more frequent when the scotoma was positioned above or to the right. 123 The clinical goal is to develop a PRL such that there is the maximum horizontal area of intact visual field extending to the right, and which is closest to the non-functioning fovea (for best VA). In the case of a symmetric, well-centred scotoma, the preference is often to move the scotoma upwards in the visual field. ...
Article
Full-text available
The purpose of the Low Vision Clinical Practice Guideline for Canadian Optometrists is to assist them in providing the best level of care in the management of patients with low vision. The guideline is based on current best evidence regarding optometric low vision rehabilitation as interpreted by an expert panel. The writing group includes optometrists from academia and private practice, representing various regions across Canada. This guideline will aid optometrists in identifying patients who require low vision rehabilitation and recommends appropriate assessment and management. As primary eye care providers, optometrists are optimally trained and qualified to identify and manage patients who would benefit from low vision rehabilitation. Optometrists, based on their geographical presence in local communities, are also well positioned to provide initial rehabilitation in a timely, effective manner, and to initiate referral for more comprehensive rehabilitation if required.
Article
Purpose Eccentric viewing training for macular disease has been performed for > 40 years, but no large studies including control groups have assessed the benefits of this training. The EFFECT (Eccentric Fixation From Enhanced Clinical Training) study is a large randomized controlled trial of 2 types of eccentric viewing training. Design Randomized controlled trial. Participants Two hundred adults with age-related macular disease. Methods Participants were randomized to either of the following: (1) a control group; (2) a group receiving supervised reading support; (3) a group receiving 3 sessions of training to optimize the use of their own preferred retinal locus; or (4) a group receiving 3 sessions of biofeedback training of a theoretically optimal trained retinal locus. All participants received standard low-vision rehabilitation. Main Outcome Measures The primary outcome was patient-reported visual task ability measured on the Activity Inventory instrument at goal level. Secondary outcomes included reading performance and fixation stability. Results There was no difference between groups on change in task ability (F(3,174) = 1.48, P = 0.22) or on any of the secondary outcome measures. Visual acuity and contrast sensitivity fell in all groups, suggesting that disease progression outweighed any benefit of training. Conclusions Eccentric viewing training did not systematically improve task ability, reading performance, or fixation stability in this study. Our results do not support the routine use of eccentric viewing training for people with progressing age-related macular disease, although this training may help people with end-stage disease. Rehabilitation of an inherently progressive condition is challenging. Financial Disclosure(s) Proprietary or commercial disclosure may be found in the Footnotes and Disclosures at the end of this article.
Chapter
We conducted 3 studies to learn about elements of satisfaction for leisure reading in low vision. Study 1 focused on alternative reading formats such as scrolling text and rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP). Study 2 explored use and satisfaction with alternative formats over time using a tablet device. Study 3 meant to assess prevalence and perceptions of tablet devices as low vision aids for sustained reading. Nine low vision participants used a tablet to read with three presentation types (Page, Scroll, Modified RSVP) over a three-week period. Study 3 involved surveys administered to 73 participants 1–3 months after a vision consultation with a low vision professional in which multiple options for reading devices were demonstrated. Studies 1 and 2 point to the need to emphasize personal preferences and satisfaction with reading devices, in addition to reading performance. Alternative presentations may be beneficial to certain people with low vision, especially those with difficulties navigating lines of text and controlling eye movements. Study 3 showed that participants were generally satisfied with their reading aids and used them frequently at a 1–3 month follow up. Participants appeared interested in tablets as a reading method, despite many having little to no experience with them prior to the study. Barriers to using a tablet were primarily cost and lack of familiarity.
Article
Two experimental reading development courses for partially sighted students were conducted at the Institute for the Blind and Partially Sighted in Copenhagen, Denmark. These students read so slowly that they could not complete their study assignments. Each course had four participants. Course 1 ran for six weeks, with two lessons a day. Course 2, which took place 1.5 years later, was a more advanced residential course. The goals of the courses were to raise and vary the participants' reading speed, to help them gain insight into the reading process, and to develop the cognitive aspects of reading. At the end of each course, the participants' proficiency in reading improved dramatically.
Article
Macular degeneration is the most widespread untreatable cause of visual impairment. Since it is age-related, it is expected to increase as the mean age of the population increases. Many individuals with this impairment wish to develop or retain reading ability, which is inhibited by the resulting central scotomas, or blind spots. The Pepper Visual Skills for Reading Test has been developed to evaluate the visual skills used in reading under these circumstances. Previously shown to be highly reliable, the Pepper VSRT is also shown to be highly valid when compared to a standardized oral reading evaluation. It can be adapted to a wide variety of clinical applications.
Article
Currently, there are no clinically practical means for accurately and precisely measuring scotomas and preferred retinal loci for visual tasks in low-vision patients. In addition, there is neither a reliable nor efficient method for training eccentric viewing at optimal retinal areas, nor for evaluating the effectiveness of the training. When scotomas involve the fovea, the low-vision patient must use an eccentric retinal area (eccentric preferred retinal locus) but the visual system may not choose an appropriate retinal area. Even patients with a paracentral scotoma may use an inappropriate retinal location, e.g., when reading part of the word may be placed into the scotoma, but the low-vision patient and rehabilitation clinician are not aware that this is occurring. Eccentric viewing training may help some of these patients compensate for their scotomas. Unfortunately, there are no means for accurately assessing the ability of eccentric viewing training in terms of optimizing the visual performance capability of an individual low-vision patient with a macular scotoma. In addition, present methods of eccentric viewing training require many man hours. Therefore, although current low-vision rehabilitation effectively incorporates compensation for impaired acuity and contrast (e.g., magnifiers and CCTVs), low-vision rehabilitation specialists have no means for effectively compensating for impaired visual function due to macular scotomas, which is a significant proportion of the low-vision population. A possible scanning laser ophthalmoscope low-vision rehabilitation system is presented as an additional tool for complete low-vision rehabilitation.
Article
The objectives of the project reported here were to discover whether the rehabilitation of reading recognition allows comprehension of print for low vision individuals who were former readers and to develop and test training strategies for enhancing comprehension for those who do not achieve good comprehension.
Article
In low vision rehabilitation, there are clinical indications that the increase in distance visual acuity (DVA) and near visual acuity (NVA) obtained by means of prescribed visual aids, in many cases cannot be used effectively in practical situations, such as in reading and writing, unless formal educational training in the use of aids and residual vision is provided. The present prospective study on two randomized groups [each of 20 patients with advanced age-related macular degeneration, one of which received formal training (the trained group, TG), while the other one was given instruction only (the untrained group, UTG], was designed to elucidate this question. DVA was 0.08 and NVA about 24 points for both groups at the outset. No patient could read TV titles (subtitles or captions) or newpaper text. Both groups were given similar aids, telescopes for distance vision, mainly hyperoculars for near vision and stronger than normal near addition for intermediate distance vision. DVA increased to about 0.4 and NVA to about 4 points for both groups. The TG was subjected to educational training in the use of the prescribed aids and in the utilization of residual vision by a low vision therapist, e.g. in the form of training of eccentric viewing (50% of the cases). The UTG was given instruction in the use of the aids by the optician, e.g. regarding correct distances. Evaluation of both groups at 1 month revealed that ability to read TV titles and newspaper text and to write letters was 70%, 100% and 85%, respectively, for the TG, whereas the figures for the UTG were significantly lower: 0%, 25% and 20%, respectively. Reading speed increased from 0 to 75.5 words/min in the TG but only from 0 to 22.6 words/min in the UTG. With training given to the UTG, all these parameters improved to levels not significantly different from those of the TG. The results clearly show that formal training in the use of aids and residual vision (e.g. training of eccentric viewing) is far more effective than mere instruction in restoring visual performance in practical situations, at least regarding elderly patients with very poor vision.
Article
Age-related macular degeneration (ARMD) is the leading cause of visual impairment in adults 65 years of age and older. ARMD often results in scotomas affecting the fovea, leading to central field loss. We used simulated scotomas in normally sighted young adults to test the impact of scotoma size and location on reading. We measured reading rate and eye movement patterns and found that the number of letters masked by the scotoma is more important than the size of the scotoma in degrees of visual angle and that forcing subjects to attend below a simulated scotoma results in better reading than when they are forced to attend to either the left or the right.
Article
1. Retinal positions for fixation were investigated in normally sighted observers, observers with peripheral vision loss due to retinitis pigmentosa, and observers with central vision loss due to macular degeneration and diabetic retinopathy. 2. Three types of pericentral fixation targets (cross, cross with a circle and four dots in a diamond shape) and a 1° fixation cross were projected on the retina at the primary position of gaze with a scanning laser ophthalmoscope until an observer reported fixation. The 1° fixation cross was also projected on the retina at secondary positions of gaze. 3. The primary gaze and secondary gaze retinal locus for fixation is operationally defined as the ellipse that encloses 95% of the retinal fixation positions found with a 1° cross at the primary position and secondary position of gaze. 4. Two sets of instructions were used to direct the observers fixation: (1) 'move your eye in order to best see the center of the target' (central visibility instructions), and (2) 'move your eye so that it is pointing directly at the center of the target' (central position instructions). 5. The retinal positions of the pericentral fixation target centers were usually within the secondary gaze retinal locus for fixation, even for the observers with central scotomas using a preferred retinal locus for fixation at a preferred eccentric retinal location. 6. The verbal instructions changed the retinal positions of the pericentral fixation target centers (i.e. the fixation behavior) in only one of the nine observers with central field loss. 7. Observers reported more difficulty in determining the diamond pericentral fixation target center even though eventually placing the diamond target center in relatively the same retinal location as the other pericentral fixation targets.
Article
Two experimental reading development courses were conducted for six partially sighted students who read so slowly that they could not complete their study assignments. At the end of each course, the participants' proficiency in reading improved dramatically. (Author)