ArticlePDF Available

Ethnic Identity and the Risk of Schizophrenia in Ethnic Minorities: A Case-Control Study

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

The high incidence of schizophrenia in immigrant ethnic groups in Western Europe may be explained by social stress associated with ethnic minority status. Positive identification with one's own ethnic group is a strong predictor of mental health in immigrants. We investigated whether negative ethnic identity is related to schizophrenia risk in non-Western immigrants. Matched case-control study of first-episode schizophrenia, including 100 non-Western immigrant cases, general hospital controls (n=100), and siblings (n=63). Conditional logistic regression analyses were used to investigate associations between schizophrenia and ethnic group identity. Cases had a negative ethnic identity more often than general hospital controls (64% and 35%, respectively, P < .001). After adjustment for marital status, level of education, unemployment, self-esteem, social support, and cannabis use, negative ethnic identity was associated with schizophrenia: odds ratio = 3.29; 95% confidence interval = 1.36-7.92. Cases significantly more often had an assimilated or a marginalized identity and less often had a separated identity. Comparisons between cases and siblings largely confirmed these findings. Negative identification with the own ethnic group may be a risk factor for schizophrenia in immigrants living in a context of social adversity.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Ethnic Identity and the Risk of Schizophrenia in Ethnic Minorities: A Case-Control
Study
Wim Veling
1–3
, Hans W. Hoek
2,4,5
, Durk Wiersma
5
, and
Johan P. Mackenbach
3
2
Research Department, Parnassia Psychiatric Institute, The Hague,
The Netherlands;
3
Department of Public Health, Erasmus Medical
Centre,Rotterdam,The Netherlands;
4
DepartmentofEpidemiology,
Columbia University, New York, NY;
5
Department of Psychiatry,
University Medical Centre Groningen, Groningen, The Netherlands
Objectives: The high incidence of schizophrenia in immi-
grant ethnic groups in Western Europe may be explained
by social stress associated with ethnic minority status. Pos-
itiveidentificationwithone’s own ethnicgroupis a strongpre-
dictor of mental health in immigrants. We investigated
whether negative ethnic identity is related to schizophrenia
risk in non-Western immigrants. Methods: Matched case-
control study of first-episode schizophrenia, including 100
non-Western immigrant cases, general hospital controls
(n5100), and siblings (n563). Conditional logistic re-
gression analyses were used to investigate associations be-
tween schizophrenia and ethnic group identity. Results:
Cases had a negative ethnic identity more often than general
hospitalcontrols (64%and 35%, respectively, P<.001).After
adjustment for marital status, level of education, unemploy-
ment, self-esteem, social support, and cannabis use, negative
ethnic identity was associated with schizophrenia: odds
ratio 53.29; 95% confidence interval 51.36–7.92. Cases
significantly more often had an assimilated or a margin-
alized identity and less often had a separated identity.
Comparisons between cases and siblings largely confirmed
these findings. Conclusions: Negative identification with
the own ethnic group may be a risk factor for schizophrenia
in immigrants living in a context of social adversity.
Key words: identity/migrants/acculturation/psychosis
Introduction
The striking finding of a very high incidence of schizo-
phrenia and other psychotic disorders among ethnic mi-
nority groups in Western Europe
1,2
remains largely
unexplained. Reviews have suggested that adverse social
experiences of ethnic minority groups may contribute to
their elevated risk, such as perceptions of discrimination
and exclusion.
3
Few studies evaluated this hypothesis,
showing that ethnic minority groups’ experiences of dis-
crimination were associated with the incidence of schizo-
phrenia in these groups,
4
perceived disadvantage
compared with other individuals in society partially
explained the excess of psychosis among the UK black
population,
5
and the risk for psychotic disorders was par-
ticularly high for members from ethnic minority groups
living in neighborhoods where their own ethnic group
comprised a small proportion of the population.
6,7
The present study investigated how such adverse social
experiences would result in individuals developing
schizophrenia. We focused on ethnic group identity be-
cause positive identification with one’s own ethnic group
has been shown to buffer negative consequences of racial
discrimination
8
and is a strong predictor of mental health
in first- and second-generation immigrants.
9,10
Strong
orientation toward the own minority group may diminish
perceptions of exclusion and disadvantage,
11
whereas
a positive ethnic minority identity is likely to be threat-
ened in those who live isolated from their own ethnic
group.
12
A second dimension of ethnic group identity is the
identity as a member of the larger society (hereafter na-
tional or Dutch identity), which is independent of ethnic
minority identity (hereafter ethnic identity).
13
An individ-
ual who retains a strong ethnic identity while also iden-
tifying with the larger society is considered to have an
integrated identity. One who has a strong ethnic identity
but a weak national identity has a separated identity,
whereas one who gives up an ethnic identity and only
has a strong national identity has an assimilated identity.
The individual who identifies neither with the own ethnic
group nor with the larger society has a marginalized iden-
tity.
9
Of these 4 identity positions, integration has been
associated with good mental health,
9,14
and marginaliza-
tion has been shown consistently to predict low self-
esteem and poor mental health.
9,11
Research of health
consequences of separation and assimilation had contra-
dictory results depending on outcome and context.
15
1
To whom correspondence should be addressed; Center for Early
Psychosis, Parnassia Psychiatric Institute, Prinsegracht 63,
2512EX The Hague, The Netherlands; tel: þ31-70-391-7160, fax:
þ31-70-391-7088, e-mail: w.veling@parnassia.nl.
Schizophrenia Bulletin vol. 36 no. 6 pp. 1149–1156, 2010
doi:10.1093/schbul/sbp032
Advance Access publication on May 8, 2009
ÓThe Author 2009. Published by Oxford University Press on behalf of the Maryland Psychiatric Research Center. All rights reserved.
For permissions, please email: journals.permissions@oxfordjournals.org.
1149
Given the pattern of a higher psychosis risk for second-
generation immigrants,
16
who are generally more assim-
ilated than immigrants of the first generation,
13
and
the findings of a lower risk for immigrants living in
high–ethnic density neighborhoods,
7
assimilation may
increase the risk of schizophrenia, and separation may
be protective.
This case-control study of first-episode schizophrenia
among non-Western ethnic minorities in The Hague, The
Netherlands, was designed to investigate associations be-
tween these factors and risk for schizophrenia. We hy-
pothesized that first- and second-generation ethnic
minorities who developed schizophrenia would identify
themselves less often and less positive with their own eth-
nic group than their siblings and nonpsychotic controls
and that they would more often have an assimilated or
marginalized identity and less often a separated or inte-
grated identity.
Methods
Classification of Ethnicity
We used the classification of ethnicity as defined by The
Netherlands’ Bureau of Statistics. If a citizen, or (one of)
his or her parents, was born abroad, he or she is assigned
to the group of people born in the same country. If the
parents were born in different foreign countries, the
country of birth of the mother determines the assignment
to a particular group.
Participants
Cases. All first- or second-generation immigrants from
non-Western countries (of which 85% from Surinam,
Morocco, Turkey, or The Netherlands Antilles), aged
18–54 years, who made first contact with a physician
in The Hague for a psychotic disorder and received a di-
agnosis of a schizophrenia-spectrum disorder (Diagnostic
and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders [Fourth Edi-
tion]: schizophrenia, schizophreniform disorder, schizo-
affective disorder) between October 1, 2000, and July
1, 2005, were eligible for the study (n=150). Case-
finding procedures and diagnostic protocol of the study
have been described elsewhere.
16
If the patient had been
adopted as a child, he or she was excluded (n=4).
Controls. For each patient, 2 control subjects were
recruited, matched for 5-year age-group, sex, and ethnicity
(including generation). They were screened for psychotic
symptoms (see ‘‘Measures’’), and were excluded if these
were present (n=5).
The first control group was recruited among the gen-
eral ethnic minority population of The Hague. To min-
imize selection bias as a result of pathways to care, the
controls were selected from immigrants who made con-
tact with nonpsychiatric secondary health-care services.
Controls were recruited from the outpatient departments
of Internal Medicine and Surgery of a general hospital.
The reasons for making contact with these departments
differed widely and included lipoma or nevus (n=15),
fracture (n=8), contusion (n=11), hemorrhoids
(n=8), sinus pilonidalis (n=5), anal fissure (n=5), in-
flammatory bowel disease (n=5), diabetes mellitus
(n=5), and other, less frequent diagnoses (n=38).
The second control group consisted of siblings of the
patients in order to (partially) control for genetic factors
and to control implicitly for unmeasured shared socioen-
vironmental confounding factors. It was not always pos-
sible to match siblings on sex and age.
All participants gave written informed consent for the
study. The study was approved by the regional ethics
committee. Structured interviews were conducted by
a resident in psychiatry (W.V.) and 4 trained research
assistants. If participants did not speak Dutch sufficiently
(n=9), 3 trained research assistants, who were native
speakers in Turkish, Kurdish, Urdu, Arabic, or Berber,
conducted the interviews. Because we expected this in ad-
vance to concern only a small minority of the sample, we
neither developed nor maintained a protocol for transla-
tion and backtranslation of the questionnaires. Partici-
pants were instructed to answer according to their
experiences in the year before illness onset, but the date
of illness onset was not assessed in a systematic way.
Measures
Ethnic and National Identity. Measures were adapted
from the International Comparative Study of Ethnocul-
tural Youth (ICSEY), a study among more than 10 000
adolescents from 30 ethnic groups in 13 countries, which
included Surinamese, Turkish, and Antillean immi-
grants in The Netherlands.
14
Identity was assessed
with the ordinal ICSEY Scale of Ethnic and National
Identity. This is a 10-item version of the Multigroup
Ethnic Identity Measure,
17
with response options rang-
ing from ‘‘strongly disagree’’ (1) to ‘‘strongly agree’’ (5),
assessing ethnic and national affirmation, sense of be-
longing, and feelings about being group member. An ex-
ample is ‘‘Being part of ethnic culture is embarrassing to
me.’’ We not only used the total scores of the Ethnic and
National Identity subscales as continuous measures but
also calculated the median scores of the Ethnic and Na-
tional Identity Scales to use these as cutoff points to
classify participants as having a positive or a negative
identity.
Also, participants were assigned to different identity
categories. Participants who scored above the median
of both ethnic identity and national identity were classi-
fied as having an integrated identity. Those with a score
above the median of ethnic identity but below the median
of national identity had a separated identity; those with
a score below the median of ethnic identity but above the
median of national identity had an assimilated identity;
W. Veling et al.
1150
and those who had a score below the median of both
measures had a marginalized identity.
Psychotic Symptoms. In control subjects, the psychosis
section of the Composite International Diagnostic Inter-
view, version 2.1,
18
was administered.
Other Measures. Associations between ethnic identity,
national identity, and schizophrenia may be confounded
and mediated by a host of factors, several of which were
explored. Self-esteem was measured with the 15-item
Rosenberg Self-esteem Scale (example: ‘‘On the whole, I
am satisfied with myself’’)
19
; the 6-item ICSEY Mastery
Scale assessed locus of control (example: ‘‘When I make
plans, I feel certain that I can make them work’’)
14
; and
the 12-item Shortened Social Support Scale (example:
‘‘How often does someone shows interest in you?’’)
20
mea-
sured perceived social support. Also, lifetime use of canna-
bis was recorded. Use was defined as more than 5 times.
Information was noted on marital status (single or
else). Socioeconomic status was assessed with level of ed-
ucation (no or primary, secondary, or higher education),
employment status (unemployed or else), and parental
social class: father’s level of occupation (according to
the classification of The Netherlands’ Bureau of Statis-
tics) and father’s level of education (no or primary, sec-
ondary, or higher education).
Key Informants. For all participants, key informants
were asked to complete a short version of the structured
interview for their relatives, which included sociodemo-
graphic information, life events, language use, racial dis-
crimination, and social behavior.
Validity and Reliability of Measures. For each ICSEY
Scale, it has been shown that it measures the same psy-
chological construct in all ethnic groups, as all Tucker’s
u’s, a measure of agreement,
21
were 0.90 or higher.
14
The
measures have shown good to excellent internal reliability
as well (Cronbach a’s >.70). Cronbach a’s in our sample
were good to excellent for Mastery (.70), Ethnic Identity
(.81), National Identity (.82), Self-esteem (.83), and Per-
ceived Social Support (.85). In 2 subsamples, we investi-
gated interrater reliability (n=23; tested because the
scales were administered by the interviewer and not filled
out by the participant) and test-retest reliability after 1
week (n=24) of the scales, with intraclass coefficients
of .85–.99 and .63–.96, respectively.
Statistical Analysis
Stata version 9.2 was used for all statistical analyses. The
pairwise matched case-control design required condi-
tional (fixed-effects) logistic regression techniques. The
regression models were fitted stepwise. First, each vari-
able was entered separately in the model. Those variables
that differed significantly between cases and control
groups (with Pvalues <.10) were selected for further
analyses. Next, the continuous and dichotomous varia-
bles of ethnic identity and national identity as well as
the 4 identity types were included with all potential con-
founding and/or mediating variables. Comparisons were
made between cases and general hospital controls (100
pairs) and between cases and sibling controls (63 pairs).
Additional analyses addressed the issue of information
bias. Scale scores of participants were compared with the
scores provided by their key informants with use of con-
ditional logistic regression.
Results
Of the 146 patients who were eligible for the study, 2
patients had deceased before the present study was con-
ducted. Twenty-six patients could not be interviewed be-
cause they had remigrated to their home country (n=5),
they were too ill during the entire study period (n=8), or
there was no current address available (n=13). Of the
118 patients who were contacted, 18 refused to partici-
pate. Thus, 100 patients were interviewed. Of the 168 sub-
jects in the general hospital control group who were
matched to the schizophrenia patients, 4 subjects were
physically too ill to be interviewed, 1 was mentally hand-
icapped, 3 were excluded because they had a psychotic
disorder, and 60 refused to participate. For 15 patients,
there was no sibling available because all siblings were
too young or lived abroad, patients had no sibling, or
patients did not know their current address. Nine patients
refused permission to contact their siblings; 2 patients
only had a sibling who had psychotic symptoms. For 11
of the remaining 74 patients, the siblings refused to partic-
ipate. Thus, siblings of 63 patients could be interviewed.
Characteristics of the study sample are shown in table 1.
Compared with controls, cases more often had single
marital status, were more often unemployed, and had
a lower level of education.
Also, cases had lower self-esteem than general hospital
controls and siblings in the year before illness onset and
more often had a lifetime history of cannabis use.
Using the continuous measure, weak ethnic identity was
strongly associated with schizophrenia in the comparison
between cases and general hospital controls (odds ratio
[OR] =1.13, 95% confidence interval [CI] =1.06–1.21,
P<.001; adjusted OR =1.12, 95% CI =1.03–1.22, P=
.006). The difference in Negative Dutch Identity Scale
score between cases and controls was not statistically sig-
nificant (OR =1.00, 95% CI =0.92–1.08, P=.983; adjusted
OR =0.89, 95% CI =0.79–1.01, P=.062).
Using the dichotomous measure, cases more often had
a negative ethnic identity than general hospital controls
(64% and 35%, respectively, P<.001, table 2). A similar
proportion of the groups had a negative Dutch identity
(table 2). After adjustment for potential confounding
and/or mediating factors, negative ethnic identity was still
significantly associated with schizophrenia (OR =3.29,
1151
Ethnic Identity and Schizophrenia
95% CI =1.36–7.92) (table 3). Also, in the final model,
a negative Dutch identity was related to a lower risk for
schizophrenia (OR =0.36, 95% CI =0.15–0.87) (table 3).
The analyses of the identity types showed that cases sig-
nificantly more often had an assimilated or a marginalized
identity and less often had a separated identity (table 4).
In the comparison between cases and siblings, the con-
tinuous measure of negative ethnic identity was related to
a higher risk for schizophrenia (OR =1.22, 95% CI =
1.09–1.37, P=.001; adjusted OR =1.30, 95% CI =
1.04–1.62, P=.019). An increase in negative Dutch iden-
tity was not significantly associated with schizophrenia
(OR =0.92, 95% CI =0.83–1.03, P=.151; adjusted
OR =0.92, 95% CI =0.96–1.10, P=.345).
The dichotomous measure of negative ethnic identity
strongly predicted schizophrenia (adjusted OR =4.33,
95% CI =1.78–10.53), whereas negative Dutch identity
was associated with a lower risk of schizophrenia (OR =
0.19, 95% CI =0.07–0.55, table 3). After adjustment for po-
tential confounding and/or mediating factors, the OR for
negative ethnic identity increased but was not statistically
significant (P=.065). Negative Dutch identity remained
to be related to a lower risk for schizophrenia (table 3).
Finally, cases had significantly less often a separated
identity than their siblings (table 4). Information from
key informants was available for 43 cases, 38 siblings,
and 44 general hospital controls. There were no statistically
significant differences between the information from the
cases and from their key informants. In the sibling and in
the general hospital control groups, participants rated their
social integration somewhat higher than their key inform-
ants did (results not shown, available on request).
Table 1. Characteristics of Study Sample by Matched Case-Control Status
a
Cases (n=100)
General Hospital
Controls (n=100) Cases (n=63)
Sibling Controls
(n=63)
Age (y) 26.6 (6.7) 27.2 (7.2) 25.9 (6.8) 26.5 (8.5)
Male sex, n(%) 74 (74) 72 (72) 50 (79) 29 (46)
b
Ethnicity, n(%)
Moroccan 29 (29) 30 (30) 20 (32) 20 (32)
Turkish 19 (19) 20 (20) 12 (19) 12 (19)
Surinamese 32 (32) 34 (34) 21 (33) 21 (33)
Other non-Western 20 (20) 17 (17) 10 (16) 10 (16)
Second generation, n(%) 36 (36) 35 (35) 27 (43) 28 (44)
Single marital status, n(%) 72 (72) 46 (46)
b
52 (83) 37 (59)
b
Level of education, n(%)
No/primary 9 (9) 11 (11) 3 (5) 6 (10)
Secondary 77 (76) 63 (63) 48 (76) 37 (59)
Higher 13 (13) 26 (26) 11 (17) 20 (32)
Occupational level of
father, n(%)
c
Low 59 (63) 46 (58) 39 (64) 39 (65)
Middle 26 (28) 28 (35) 17 (28) 15 (25)
High 8 (9) 6 (8) 5 (8) 6 (10)
Level of education of
father, n(%)
d
No/primary 41 (57) 49 (62) 21 (47) 30 (56)
Secondary 25 (35) 22 (28) 19 (43) 18 (33)
Higher 6 (8) 8 (10) 4 (9) 6 (11)
Unemployed, n(%) 17 (17) 9 (9) 13 (21) 3 (5)
e
Cannabis use, n(%) 59 (59) 21 (21)
b
20 (32) 13 (21)
b
Self-esteem 53.68 (12.12) 60.28 (9.97)
b
53.65 (12.26) 61.52 (10.13)
b
Mastery 23.73 (5.21) 24.76 (4.30) 24.73 (4.56) 25.87 (3.66)
Perceived social support 27.95 (8.12) 33.80 (5.86)
b
28.71 (8.30) 31.10 (6.81)
a
Means (SDs), unless otherwise specified.
b
P<.005, Wald test, conditional logistic regression analysis.
c
Information missing for 31 (11.7%) participants.
d
Information missing for 59 (22.3%) participants.
e
P<.05, Wald test, conditional logistic regression analysis.
1152
W. Veling et al.
Discussion
In this case-control study of first-episode schizophrenia
among non-Western ethnic minorities, negative
identification with the own ethnic group was associated
with schizophrenia. Individuals who developed schizo-
phrenia identified themselves less positive with their
own ethnic group than general hospital controls in
the year before illness onset. There were no consistent
associations between negative Dutch identity and
schizophrenia risk, but cases had more often an assim-
ilated or a marginalized identity than the matched con-
trols and less often a separated identity.
Comparisons between cases and their siblings largely
confirmed these findings. Although this case-sibling de-
sign only partially controls for genetic factors, the simi-
larity of the results in the 2 control groups makes it
unlikely that genetic vulnerability for schizophrenia
can account for the findings.
These results are consistent with the hypothesis that
identification with the own ethnic group, but not identi-
fication with the majority group, may protect from
schizophrenia. Positive identification with the own ethnic
group is not intrinsically protective; however, its value
depends upon the social context. Belonging to an ethnic
minority group subjected to discrimination and negative
stereotyping has previously been associated with the in-
cidence of schizophrenia
4
and represents a threat to self-
esteem and social identity.
11
Individuals may respond to
this threat by asserting identification with their group and
Table 2. Measures of Identity and Risk for Schizophrenia by Matched Case-Control Status
a
Cases
(n=100)
General Hospital
Controls (n=100) OR (95% CI)
Cases
(n=63)
Sibling Controls
(n=63) OR (95% CI)
Negative ethnic
identity, n(%)
b
64 (64) 35 (35) 3.42 (1.80–6.50) 38 (60) 18 (29) 4.33 (1.78–10.53)
Negative Dutch
identity, n(%)
b
47 (47) 53 (53) 0.76 (0.42–1.38) 30 (48) 48 (76) 0.19 (0.07–0.55)
Note: OR, odds ratio; CI, confidence interval.
a
Differences between groups tested with Wald tests in conditional logistic regression analysis.
b
Median used as cutoff on scale scores of affective ethnic and Dutch identity.
Table 3. Effect of Potential Confounding/Mediating Factors in the Relationship Between Schizophrenia and Ethnic Minority/Dutch
Identity
a
Cases vs General Hospital Controls (100 Pairs) Cases vs Sibling Controls
b
(63 Pairs)
OR 95% CI POR 95% CI P
Negative ethnic
identity
Unadjusted 3.42 1.80–6.50 .000 4.26 1.57–11.54 .004
Adjustment for
Marital status 4.09 1.96–8.52 .000 4.72 1.52–14.62 .007
Level of education 3.69 1.89–7.21 .000 4.14 1.48–11.60 .007
Unemployment 3.64 1.86–7.09 .000 4.66 1.52–14.26 .007
Self-esteem 3.08 1.57–6.06 .001 2.47 0.81–7.50 .111
Social support 2.68 1.33–5.40 .006 4.28 1.47–12.53 .008
Cannabis use 3.05 1.58–5.88 .001 4.06 1.50–11.02 .006
All the above 3.29 1.36–7.92 .008 6.39 0.89–45.93 .065
Negative Dutch identity
Unadjusted 0.76 0.42–1.38 .367 0.23 0.07–0.74 .014
Adjustment for
Marital status 0.68 0.35–1.30 .239 0.16 0.04–0.61 .007
Level of education 0.78 0.43–1.44 .428 0.23 0.07–0.74 .013
Unemployment 0.68 0.36–1.26 .217 0.26 0.07–0.95 .042
Self-esteem 0.64 0.33–1.24 .183 0.20 0.05–0.75 .017
Social support 0.49 0.24–1.00 .050 0.21 0.06–0.71 .011
Cannabis use 0.76 0.40–1.44 .395 0.21 0.07–0.69 .010
All the above 0.36 0.15–0.87 .023 0.14 0.02–0.93 .042
Note: OR, odds ratio; CI, confidence interval.
a
Conditional logistic regression, differences tested for statistical significance with Wald tests.
b
All associations adjusted for sex because matching for sex was not always possible.
1153
Ethnic Identity and Schizophrenia
by seeking positive distinctiveness from the majority
group, which enhances self-esteem,
22
prevents or buffers
stress,
8
and has been associated with psychological well-
being
9,10
(an example of this strategy is the ‘‘Black is beau-
tiful’’ movement in the 1960s in the United States). An op-
posite response to this threat is trying to downplay ethnic
identity, striving to leave the low-status minority group
and to join the dominant group.
11
The boundaries be-
tween ethnic groups are difficult to cross, however, which
meansthatthis strategy often increasesratherthan resolves
the threat.
13
It is likely to be associated with feelings of hu-
miliation and with experiences of an undeserved gap be-
tween aspirations and achievements.
23
The social stress
resulting from this social defeat is a severe cognitive and
emotional challenge, which may exceed the coping ability
of individuals with a genetic vulnerability to schizophrenia,
who often have impaired executive function. When sub-
jected to such a severe challenge, they may be more likely
to develop the disorder.
23
Results from animal experiments
suggest that social stress may induce changes in the brain
that resemble those in schizophrenia. Repeated exposure
to social stress enhances the behavioral response of rats
to dopamine agonists and leads to mesolimbic dopami-
nergic hyperactivity,
24
which has been implicated in the
pathogenesis of schizophrenia.
25
Confounding and/or Mediating Factors
The effects of several social and psychological factors on
the associations between schizophrenia and identity were
investigated by adding these separately to the regression
model. The influence of ethnic identity on schizophrenia
risk was partly attenuated by cannabis use, self-esteem,
and perceived social support. Cannabis use may be a con-
founding as well as a mediating factor. It not only has
been related to the onset of schizophrenia
26
but may
also be a behavioral consequence of the social stress
of negative ethnic identity. Indeed, in our total sample,
cannabis use was correlated to negative affective ethnic
identity (r=.22, P<.005).
Lack of ethnic identification may also lead to social
isolation and less social support (correlation between
negative affective ethnic identity and social support: r=
.22, P<.005). Social support increases access to nor-
malizing explanations for anomalous perceptual experi-
ences and abnormal beliefs that are present in individuals
at high risk for developing psychosis.
27
Whereas social
isolation may contribute to the acceptance of a psychotic
appraisal of these early abnormal mental states, a social
network may have a normalizing function, thus prevent-
ing transition into psychosis.
28
Limitations
Several limitations are inherent to the case-control de-
sign. First, because the interviews were conducted
(shortly) after the first episode of schizophrenia, the
results may have been influenced by the illness. We
have tried to minimize this potential bias by instructing
the patients that the interview concerned the period be-
fore the onset of illness. Still, it is possible that (the early
stage of) the illness leads to negative feelings about being
ethnic and to more positive feelings about being Dutch,
for instance in patients who blame their problems to their
membership of an ethnic group or who feel rejected by
this group.
This issue of reverse causality also applies to the poten-
tial mediating factors. For instance, low social support
from family and other members of the own ethnic group
may lead to a weak and negative ethnic identity.
Second, it is difficult to assess experiences, behaviors,
and opinions accurately in retrospect. This applies to all
participants because we interviewed the controls on the
same time period as the case they were matched to,
but problems with recall are likely to be larger for cases
than for controls as a result of cognitive impairments
caused by the illness. Additional analyses showed that
there were no significant differences between scores of
cases and their key informants (available on request),
suggesting that recall bias cannot explain the results.
Table 4. ORs of Schizophrenia for Identity Types; Conditional Logistic Regression
Identity Type
a
Cases vs General Hospital Controls
b
Cases vs Sibling Controls
c
OR 95% CI POR 95% CI P
Integrated 0.54 0.26–1.11 .10 4.69 0.81–27.91 .09
Separated 0.17 0.06–0.47 .001 0.09 0.02–0.42 .002
Assimilated 4.68 1.82–12.04 .001 5.55 0.75–41.00 .09
Marginalized 2.28 1.06–4.91 .04 3.05 0.84–11.09 .09
Note: OR, odds ratio; CI, confidence interval.
a
Median used as cutoff on scale scores of affective ethnic and Dutch identity. Integrated identity indicates high ethnic and high Dutch
identity, separated is high ethnic and low Dutch identity, assimilated is low ethnic and high Dutch identity, marginalized is low ethnic
and low Dutch identity.
b
One hundred pairs, associations adjusted for marital status, level of education, and unemployment.
c
Sixty-three pairs, associations adjusted for sex, marital status, level of education, and unemployment.
1154
W. Veling et al.
The sample size was relatively small. Comparisons be-
tween cases and siblings may have been underpowered
because only 63 siblings participated, and the matched
case-control design required conditional analyses. This
had large consequences for the statistical power, as the
pairwise analysis implied that the data of 37 cases could
not be used in these comparisons.
All consecutive first-episode schizophrenia cases be-
tween 2000 and 2005 were eligible for the study, but
not all patients participated. It is conceivable that those
individuals who were oriented more toward their own
ethnic group lost contact with psychiatric services in
the early phase of treatment or that they refused to par-
ticipate. However, the same selection bias would occur in
the control groups.
The general hospital controls may not have been rep-
resentative for the general immigrant population, but
the choice for a control group selected from immigrants
who made contact with nonpsychiatric secondary
health-care services reduced selection bias as a result
of pathways to care, as the schizophrenia cases were
also recruited from secondary psychiatric services. Still,
it may be argued that immigrants who seek help for their
physical health problems have a more positive attitude
toward Dutch society than schizophrenia patients,
a proportion of whom has been compelled into contact
with mental health services. This type of bias would lead
to an underestimation of the effect of negative ethnic
identity.
In addition, the very diverse complaints for which the
controls made contact make it very unlikely that their so-
matic illness would be related to ethnic identity.
We had also included the ICSEY measures of accultur-
ation strategies in the study,
14
measuring preference for
assimilation, integration, separation, and marginaliza-
tion in 5 domains of life: cultural traditions, language,
marriage, social activities, and friends. The scales, how-
ever, had low Cronbach a’s in our study sample (.34–.58),
indicating that these measures were not very reliable.
Therefore, we did not report the results in the main anal-
yses, although the results support the other findings, as
cases had significantly higher scores on assimilation
and marginalization than both control groups (results
available on request).
Previous Findings
These results are consistent with reports of a higher inci-
dence of schizophrenia among ethnic minorities living in
neighborhoods where their own ethnic group comprises
a small proportion of the population.
6,7
Those who live
in low–ethnic density neighborhoods must contend with
the triple burden of increased exposure to prejudice, re-
duced social support, and fewer possibilities for positive
ethnic identification,
12
factors that are likely to increase
the social stress of minority status.
Previous studies have found variable and modest asso-
ciations of low family socioeconomic status and incidence
of schizophrenia
29
and have suggested that socioeco-
nomic disadvantage may contribute to the increased in-
cidence among immigrants.
30
In our data, there were no
significant differences in parental socioeconomic status
between the groups, but it was very low in all groups.
Low socioeconomic status may represent a situation of
social exclusion,
29
particularly for individuals who com-
pare themselves predominantly with the advantaged ma-
jority group.
11,13
Conclusions
This study is the first to investigate associations between
ethnic/national identity and schizophrenia in ethnic mi-
norities and found that negative ethnic identity may be
related to schizophrenia risk. These results should be
interpreted with caution, given the complexity of the con-
cepts, the inherent difficulties in measuring these, and the
methodological limitations of the case-control design.
Future research might include identity measures in stud-
ies among high-risk individuals in order to investigate
whether weak ethnic identity predicts transition into psy-
chosis. If weak ethnic identity is a risk factor for schizo-
phrenia, preventive interventions may be developed to
empower young members from ethnic minority groups.
Funding
The Netherlands Organization for Health Research and
Development (100-002-009).
Acknowledgments
Declaration of interest: None. Use of the ICSEY
questionnaires by courtesy of the ICSEY research
group. We thank Han Entzinger, professor of Migration
and Integration Studies, Faculty of Social Sciences,
Erasmus University Rotterdam, for his comments on
a previous version of this article.
References
1. Fearon P, Kirkbride JB, Morgan C, et al. Incidence of
schizophrenia and other psychoses in ethnic minority groups:
results from the MRC AESOP study. Psychol Med. 2006;
36:1541–1550.
2. Selten JP, Veen N, Feller W, et al. Incidence of psychotic dis-
orders in immigrant groups to The Netherlands. Br J Psychi-
atry. 2001;178:367–372.
3. Cantor-Graae E, Selten JP. Schizophrenia and migration:
a meta-analysis and review. Am J Psychiatry. 2005;162:12–24.
4. Veling W, Selten JP, Susser E, Laan W, Mackenbach JP,
Hoek HW. Discrimination and the incidence of psychotic dis-
orders among ethnic minorities in The Netherlands. Int J Epi-
demiol. 2007;36:761–768.
1155
Ethnic Identity and Schizophrenia
5. Cooper C, Morgan C, Byrne M, et al. Perceptions of disadvan-
tage, ethnici ty and psychosis. Br J Psychiatry. 2008;192:185–190 .
6. Boydell J, van Os J, McKenzie K, et al. Incidence of schizo-
phrenia in ethnic minorities in London: ecological study into
interactions with environment. BMJ. 2001;323:1336–1338.
7. Veling W, Susser E, Van Os J, Mackenbach JP, Selten JP,
Hoek HW. Ethnic density of neighborhoods and incidence
of psychotic disorders among immigrants. Am J Psychiatry.
2008;165:66–73.
8. Branscombe NR, Schmitt MT, Harvey RD. Perceiving perva-
sive discrimination among African Americans: implications
for group identification and well-being. J Pers Soc Psychol.
1999;77:135–149.
9. Phinney JS. Ethnic identity and acculturation. In: Chun KM,
Balls Organista P, Marin G, eds. Acculturation: Advances in
Theory, Measurement and Applied Research. Washington,
DC: American Psychological Association; 2003:63–82.
10. Sam DL. Psychological adaptation of adolescents with immi-
grant backgrounds. J Soc Psychol. 2000;140:5–25.
11. Tajfel H, Turner J. An integrative theory of intergroup con-
flict. In: Hogg MA, Abrams D, eds. Intergroup Relations:
Essential Readings. Philadelphia, Pa: Psychology Press; 2001:
94–109.
12. Halpern D. Minorities and mental health. Soc Sci Med.
1993;36:597–607.
13. Ward C, Bochner S, Furnham A. Social identification theo-
ries. In: Ward C, Bochner S, Furnham A, eds. The Psychology
of Culture Shock. Hove, UK: Routledge; 2001:98–121.
14. Berry JW, Phinney JS, Sam DL, Vedder PE. Immigrant Youth
in Cultural Transition. Acculturation, Identity, and Adaptation
Across National Contexts. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates; 2006.
15. Balls Organista P, Organista KC, Kurasaki K. The relationship
between acculturation and ethnic minority mental health. In:
Chun KM, Balls Organista P, Marin G, eds. Acculturation:
Advances in Theory, Measurement, and Applied Research.
Washington, DC: American Psychological Association;
2003:139–162.
16. Veling W, Selten JP, Veen N, Laan W, Blom JD, Hoek HW.
Incidence of schizophrenia among ethnic minorities in The
Netherlands: a four-year first-contact study. Schizophr Res.
2006;86:189–193.
17. Phinney JS. The Multigroup Ethnic Identity Measure: a new
scale for use with adolescents and young adults from diverse
groups. J Adolesc Res. 1992;7:156–176.
18. Smitten MH, Smeets RMW, Van den Brink W. Composite In-
ternational Diagnostic Interview (CIDI), Version 2.1. Amster-
dam, The Netherlands: World Health Organization; 1998.
19. Rosenberg M. Society and the Adolescent Self-Image. Prince-
ton, NJ: Princeton University Press; 1965.
20. Van Eijk LM, van Kempen GIJM, van Sonderen FLP. A
short scale for measuring social support in the elderly: the
SSL12-I. Tijdschr Gerontol Geriatr. 1994;25:192–196.
21. Van de Vijver FJR, Leung K. Methods and Data Analysis for
Cross-Cultural Research. Thousand Oaks, Calif: Sage; 1997.
22. Crocker J, Major B. Social stigma and self-esteem: the self-
protective properties of stigma. Psychol Rev. 1989;96:
608–630.
23. Selten JP, Cantor-Graae E. Social defeat: risk factor for
schizophrenia? Br J Psychiatry. 2005;187:101–102.
24. Tidey JW, Miczek KA. Social defeat stress selectively alters
mesocorticolimbic dopamine release: an in vivo microdialysis
study. Brain Res. 1996;721:140–149.
25. Laruelle M. Dopamine transmission in the schizophrenic brain.
In: Hirsch SR, Weinberger D, eds. Schizophrenia. Oxford, UK:
Blackwell; 2003:365–387.
26. Henquet C, Krabbendam L, Spauwen J, et al. Prospective co-
hort study of cannabis use, predisposition for psychosis, and
psychotic symptoms in young people. BMJ. 2005;330:11.
27. Hodges A, Byrne M, Grant E, Johnstone E. People at risk of
schizophrenia. Sample characteristics of the first 100 cases in
the Edinburgh High-Risk Study. Br J Psychiatry.
1999;174:547–553.
28. Garety P, Kuipers E, Fowler D, Freeman D, Bebbington P. A
cognitive model of the positive symptoms of psychosis. Psy-
chol Med. 2001;31:189–195.
29. Wicks S, Hjern A, Gunnell D, Lewis G, Dalman C. Social ad-
versity in childhood and the risk of developing psychosis:
a national cohort study. Am J Psychiatry. 2005;162:
1652–1657.
30. Hjern A, Wicks S, Dalman C. Social adversity contributes to
high morbidity in psychoses in immigrants––a national co-
hort study in two generations of Swedish residents. Psychol
Med. 2004;34:1025–1033.
1156
W. Veling et al.
... Recent evidence suggests that these findings are related to the accumulation of social adversity experiences . In fact, many migrant and ethnic minority communities face important life obstacles ranging from traumatic experiences related to the migration process, to socio-deprivation, discrimination (Pearce et al., 2019;Reininghaus et al., 2010), cultural distance (Jongsma et al., 2020) and segregation (Baker et al., 2021;Veling et al., 2010) in the host country. Migrants and ethnic minority populations also face more obstacles to accessing mental healthcare services, are more frequently hospitalized under constraint (Anderson et al., 2014), disengage from treatment more often (Ouellet-Plamondon et al., 2015) and may have poorer illness outcomes (Golay et al., 2019;Jones et al., 2019;Morgan et al., 2017). ...
Article
Several migrant populations have been identified worldwide as high-risk groups for psychosis because of their experience of social adversity. Recent evidence suggests that the local contexts in which these populations live should be addressed in their complexity to take into account individual and larger societal environmental aspects. This study aimed to assess the lived experiences of a group of migrant Cape Verdean patients, who had been recently hospitalized for a first episode of psychosis in a mental health service on the outskirts of Lisbon, Portugal. The study used Photovoice, a qualitative participatory research method in which people's experiences are documented through photography. Six individuals were recruited, and five weekly sessions were conducted to collect data that were analyzed thematically. Emergent themes addressed two main categories of well-being and illness. Participant concepts of well-being were rooted in a definition of freedom encompassing cultural expression, conveyed by familiar environments and supporting communities. Cultural differences may be experienced as important obstacles for well-being and can be associated with feelings of oppression and guilt. Participants’ accounts focused on positive aspects of life despite illness and on personal concepts of recovery. The study findings contribute to knowledge of the dynamics of migrants’ social experience and underscore the importance of socially and culturally informed mental healthcare institutions.
... • advanced paternal age (>40 years) at conception (odds ratio 1.47), 21 • presence of obstetric or perinatal complications (odds ratio 3.66), 22 • whether the participant was born in the winter or spring (odds ratio 1.21), 23 • upbringing in an urban area (odds ratio 1.19), 24 • childhood history of physical abuse, sexual abuse or neglect (odds ratio 2.66), 25 • death of a parent when the participant was a child (odds ratio 2.70), 26 • history of cannabis abuse (odds ratio 1.37), 27 and • being a member of an ethnic minority (odds ratio 1.40). 28 Environmental risk factors for mood disorders: ...
Article
Full-text available
Background Both genetic and environmental factors are important in etiology of mental disorders. Calculating polyenviromic risk/protective scores provides an updated perspective in research on the environmental causes of psychiatric disorders. We aimed to compare environmental risk and protective factors in patients with psychosis or a mood disorder (PSYCH+MOOD) and those with an anxiety disorder (ANX). Methods We administered the internationally accepted questionnaire from the EUropean Network of National Schizophrenia Networks Studying Gene-Environment Interactions (EU-GEI) study, enriched with mood and anxiety disorder-relevant measures, to patients at two large university hospitals in the Czech Republic. Results Ninety-four PSYCH+MOOD patients (average age 42.5 years; 46 males) and 52 ANX patients (average age 47.2 years; 17 males) participated. Neither polyenviromic risk score nor polyenviromic protective score differed significantly between PSYCH+MOOD and ANX groups (p = 0.149; p = 0.466, respectively). Conclusion Scientific validity of the polyenviromic risk/protective score construct must still be demonstrated in large psychiatric samples, ideally in prospective studies. Nevertheless, researchers have already started to investigate environmental factors in the etiology of mental disorders in their complexity, similarly to polygenic risk scores.
... There are, however, studies that show that group cohesion, that is, in which social networks are reinforced and not the breaking of ties, may be associated with more favorable results in the incidence of schizophrenia. In fact, everything indicates that negative ethnic identity, that is, the lack of affinity with one's own ethnic group, implies a greater risk of developing psychosis (Veling et al, 2010). Bhugra et al. (2010) compared the incidence of schizophrenia in the United Kingdom among African, Caribbean and Asian immigrants, concluding that the incidence was lower in migrants of Asian origin considered "more traditional", in which the socio-family structure acts as a "buffer". ...
... Being a predictor of Positive Schizotypy, the one-child variable, in addition to that the males may have the potential feeling of being a minority among a majority of females in such Normal University types, supports the result that males scored higher on Positive Schizotypy. Being a minority (including sexual identity and gender minority) has a higher risk of developing Schizophrenic tendencies [e.g., see (130)(131)(132)(133)]. Also, online games, in which males are engaged in higher than females are correlated positively with higher Disorganized Schizotypy (supporting the higher scores on disorganized schizotypy for male participants), and shopping is correlated negatively with Negative Shizotypy (males scored lower than females). ...
Article
Full-text available
The pandemic-related lifestyle has potentially imposed crucial disturbances on daily and long-term activities, which, in turn, were associated with thought disturbance. This study investigates how the characteristics of the activity system during pandemic-related restrictions are associated with other psychomental aspects. By focusing on PTSD, mentalization, and schizotypal personality, and by inquiring about the main components of the activity system of 852 college students (Zhengzhou, Henan, China)- including the goals orienting their activity, goals' terms and types, the motivation levels and sources, the activity type and engagement time, the flow of the activity, and how due to pandemic lifestyle-results revealed that the activity system's components have significant associations with PTSD, reflective function, and schizotypal traits. Additionally, some of the activity system's elements have a significant predictive role regarding schizotypal traits. The study considered that the life narrative during the pandemic has been disturbed; hence, this may have a crucial effect on mind coherence. Additionally, the outcomes from the pandemic context will support mental health interventions in other similar contexts where the life narrative is severely affected.
... These findings are in line with the existing literature. The process of adapting to different cultures was described by Berry (1997) as 'acculturation,' and the difficulties deriving from this process referred to as 'acculturative stress,' which has been associated with psychiatric problems (Devylder et al., 2013;Veling et al., 2010). In line with Berry's acculturation theory, Phinney et al. (2001) described ethnic identity as 'the aspect of acculturation that focuses on the subject's sense of belonging to a group or culture.' ...
Article
Migrants and their offspring are at increased risk of developing mental disorders. Previous research has shown associations between adverse social factors (e.g., discrimination, lack of social support) and mental health problems in migrants, but it is unknown how these associations are understood by migrants themselves. In this study, we aimed to gain explorative insight into the way young Moroccan-Dutch people experience their social environment, and how they relate this social environment to the development of mental health problems. At www.marokko.nl , the largest online discussion platform for young Moroccan-Dutch people, contributors discuss a broad variety of subjects, including societal, cultural, religious, and mental health issues. Combining deductive and inductive approaches to qualitative data analysis, we analysed 22 forum discussions at marokko.nl about mental health problems, after which data saturation was reached. Contributors described feeling isolated and experiencing discrimination in their social environment. Contributor comments identified social challenges arising from Dutch society, Dutch culture (e.g., being too individualistic), Moroccan culture (e.g., strict parenting style), and living between these two cultures. These social challenges are perceived to be associated with mental health problems. Furthermore, we created a model describing the different types of explanations contributors used for mental health problems, being: religious (e.g., possession); medical (i.e., a bio-psycho-social cause); or a combination of both. This model can help clinicians in delivering culturally sensitive mental health care. Lastly, this study shows the taboo on mental health problems in the Moroccan-Dutch population and the opportunity to open up in the online environment.
... In addition, many studies have focused on the presence or the absence of psychiatric symptoms as indicators of the influence of ethnic identity development. Stronger ethnic identity has been associated with fewer depressive and anxiety symptoms among Black and Latino/a American adults (Burnett-Zeigler et al., 2013;Williams et al., 2012) and adolescents (Rivas-Drake, Sellers et al., 2003;Smith & Silva, 2011), and it has been associated with reduced psychotic symptoms among racial and ethnic minorities Espinosa et al., 2020;Veling et al., 2010). ...
Article
Full-text available
Objective: Serious mental illnesses (SMI) often occur during early adulthood, just as young people are developing important aspects of their identity that can affect their recovery. Positive ethnic identity development is associated with stress coping and psychological well-being in young people. But, there is limited research to indicate how individual experiences of belonging and attachment to one's ethnic group influence personal recovery processes among young adults living with SMI. Method: Young adults living with SMI (95% identified as ethnic/racial minorities) were recruited from four outpatient psychiatric rehabilitation programs (N = 83). Multivariate regressions were used to examine relationships between predictors (demographics, psychiatric symptomatology, ethnic identity) and the dependent variables (perceived stress and personal recovery). Results: A stronger, more developed ethnic identity and fewer depressive symptoms were associated with higher ratings of personal recovery. Increases in psychiatric symptoms predicted increased perceived stress. Post hoc analyses showed that Black, Latino/a, and multiracial study participants' ethnic identity ratings were similar to those of same ethnic/racial group of young adults without SMI. Conclusions and implications for practice: Ethnic identity development could be a significant psychosocial factor shaping mental health recovery among minority young people living with SMI. Several factors associated with psychological well-being among ethnic and racial minority youth may account for this, including adaptive coping, social support, and a buffering effect against racism. Our findings indicate that assessing and developing a young person's ethnic identity-related strengths and resources as a means for improving the personalization of recovery services and enhancing the quality of care. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).
Article
Full-text available
Epidemiological studies that examined the association between migration and schizophrenia can provide important insights about the social risk factors involved in the outbreak of the severe mental illnesses. The aim of this article is to present a review of the literature, which aimed to examine an association between migration and schizophrenia and to compare findings from Israel and other Western countries. A narrative literature review from the last two decades (2000-2020) was conducted using PubMed, PsycInfo and Google Scholar databases. During the review, 64 articles that examined the relationship between migration and schizophrenia worldwide and in Israel were found and analyzed. Findings from studies in Western Europe have shown that first- and second-generation immigrants were two-to-three times more at risk of developing schizophrenia and other psychotic disorders than were native-born individuals. The risk was even higher among immigrants from developing countries with physical and culturally visible characteristics differentiating them from the native-born population. Studies conducted in Israel have found that first-generation immigrants had a higher risk of schizophrenia, but this risk decreased in second-generation immigrants. The social defeat/exclusion theory seems to best explain the association between migration and schizophrenia in Israel and other Western countries. Migration policy makers and mental health professionals should be aware of the impact of post-migration social adversities on the increased risk of schizophrenia in Israel, especially for excluded groups such as Ethiopian immigrants. Increased social support and better access to mental health care services are essential steps towards minimizing the consequences of this phenomenon.
Article
This article discusses how people living with schizophrenia experience, understand, and respond to their urban environment. Our study relies on experiential photo-voice data gathered with a sample of six people diagnosed with schizophrenia and living in non-institutional settings in Montréal, Canada, to identify how individuals in this community perceive the urban landscape. We adopt a therapeutic landscapes' framework that explores the urban fabric at three levels: physical, social, and symbolic. Research participants identified both health-denying and health-enhancing places within ordinary urban landscapes. Landscapes identified as health-denying are characterized by environmental stressors and loss of control, with construction sites an example highlighted by participants. Healing and restorative landscapes, as identified by participants, were physically attractive or quiet, socially safe and welcoming, and symbolically affirmative of one's identity, all factors worthy of further study. The findings are also policy-relevant: they suggest that people living with schizophrenia and their clinicians can develop strategies to make health-enhancing uses of urban landscapes; and that urban policies and practices can foster urban environments conducive to enhanced health and well-being, both for the community of people living with schizophrenia and the wider population of urban dwellers.
Article
The Bipolar-Schizophrenia Network for Intermediate Phenotypes (B-SNIP) has invested in the collection and use of multiple biomarkers in individuals with psychosis. We expect psychosis biology and its distinctive types to be reflected in the biomarkers, as they are the ‘behaviors’ of the brain. Like infectious diseases, we expect the etiologies of these biomarker-driven entities to be multiple and complex. Biomarkers have not yet been annotated with disease characteristics and need to be. As a model, we seek to adopt aspects of the Framingham Heart Study (FHS) to guide and organize these observations.
Article
Full-text available
The processes involved in well-being maintenance among African Americans who differed in their attributions to prejudice were examined. A rejection–identification model was proposed where stable attributions to prejudice represent rejection by the dominant group. This results in a direct and negative effect on well-being. The model also predicts a positive effect on well-being that is mediated by minority group identification. In other words, the generally negative consequences of perceiving oneself as a victim of racial prejudice can be somewhat alleviated by identification with the minority group. Structural equation analyses provided support for the model and ruled out alternative theoretical possibilities. Perceiving prejudice as pervasive produces effects on well-being that are fundamentally different from those that may arise from an unstable attribution to prejudice for a single negative outcome.
Chapter
Organizational Identity presents the classic works on organizational identity alongside more current thinking on the issues. Ranging from theoretical contributions to empirical studies, the readings in this volume address the key issues of organizational identity, and show how these issues have developed through contributions from such diverse fields of study as sociology, psychology, management studies and cultural studies. The readings examine questions such as how organizations understand who they are, why organizations develop a sense of identity and belonging where the boundaries of identity lie and the implications of postmodern and critical theories' challenges to the concept of identity as deeply-rooted and authentic. Includes work by: Stuart Albert, Mats Alvesson, Blake E. Ashforth, Marilynn B. Brewer, George Cheney, Lars Thoger Christensen, C.H. Cooley, Kevin G. Corley, Barbara Czarniawska, Janet M. Dukerich, Jane E. Dutton, Kimberly D. Elsbach, Wendi Gardner, Linda E. Ginzela, Dennis A. Gioia, E. Goffman, Karen Golden-Biddle, Mary Jo Hatch, Roderick M. Kramer, Fred Rael, G.H. Mead, Michael G. Pratt, Anat Rafaeli, Hayagreeva Rao, Majken Schultz, Howard S. Schwartz, Robert I. Sutton, Henri Taijfel, John Turner, David A. Wherren, and Hugh Willmott. Intended to provide easy access to this material for students of organizational identity, it will also be of interest more broadly to students of business, sociology and psychology.
Conference Paper
The processes involved in well-being maintenance among African Americans who differed in their attributions to prejudice were examined. A rejection-identification model was proposed where stable attributions to prejudice represent rejection by the dominant group. This results in a direct and negative effect on well-being. The model also predicts a positive effect on well-being that is mediated by minority group identification. In other words, the generally negative consequences of perceiving oneself as a victim of racial prejudice can be somewhat alleviated by identification with the minority group. Structural equation analyses provided support for the model and ruled out alternative theoretical possibilities. Perceiving prejudice as pervasive produces effects on well-being that are fundamentally different from those that may arise from an unstable attribution to prejudice for a single negative outcome.
Article
Ethnic identity is an important component of the self-concept and, like other aspects of identity, can be particularly salient during adolescence. Most research on ethnic identity has focused on the unique elements that distinguish particular ethnic groups. However, it is important as well to study and compare ethnic identity and its correlates across groups. This article presents a questionnaire measure of ethnic identity based on the elements of ethnic identity that are common across groups, so that it can be used with all ethnic groups. The questionnaire was administered to 417 high school students and 136 college students from ethnically diverse schools. Reliability, assessed by Cronbach 's alpha, was .81 for the high school sample and .9Ofor the college sample. The relationship of ethnic identity to various demographic variables and to self-esteem was examined. The measure can be used to examine similarities and differences in ethnlic identity and its correlates among youths from different ethnic groups.
Book
(from the cover) Heavy migration patterns, the globalization of markets, and increased cross-cultural communications have made cross-cultural research a necessity in the behavioral and social sciences. In "Methods and Data Analysis for Cross-Cultural Research," the authors have developed a guide for graduate students and professionals that presents cross-cultural methodology in a practical light. The central focus is primarily on the design and analysis of quasi-experiments, which is the dominant framework for cross-cultural research. This volume presents an overview of tools of cross-cultural research and illustrates the most meaningful techniques in feature boxes, complete with sample data. (cover) This guide will be of interest to researchers in the fields of clinical/counseling psychology, social work, research methods, sociology, ethnic studies, and social psychology. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved) (cover)
Article
The purpose of this chapter is to provide an overview and analysis of research on the relationship between acculturation and mental health among African Americans, American Indians, Asian Americans, and Latinos. A review of the literature reveals that research in this area has been uneven across major ethnic groups in the US. Research has tended to focus on Latinos, but far less work has addressed this issue as it affects African Americans and American Indians. Therefore, this chapter includes an analysis of the research of these ethnic groups that begins with basic issues of conceptualization and measurement of distress and acculturation in African Americans and American Indians, as well as the insights gained from the more methodically sophisticated studies of Asian Americans and large-scale prevalence studies of Latino populations. It is concluded that much more research is needed to better understand the relationship of the acculturation process in various cultures and its relationship to mental health. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)