ArticlePDF AvailableLiterature Review

Spatial reorientation in large and small enclosures: Comparative and developmental perspectives

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Several vertebrate species, including humans, following passive spatial disorientation appear to be able to reorient themselves by making use of the geometric shape of the environment (i.e., metric properties of surfaces and directional sense). In some circumstances, reliance on such purely geometric information can overcome the use of local featural cues (landmarks). The relative use of geometric and non-geometric information seems to depend upon, among other factors, the size of the experimental space. Evidence in non-human animals and in human infants for primacy in encoding either geometric or landmark information depending on the size of the environment is reviewed, together with possible theoretical accounts of this phenomenon.
Content may be subject to copyright.
REVIEW
Spatial reorientation in large and small enclosures:
comparative and developmental perspectives
Cinzia Chiandetti Æ Giorgio Vallortigara
Received: 18 April 2007 / Revised: 12 October 2007 / Accepted: 3 January 2008 / Published online: 15 January 2008
Ó Marta Olivetti Belardinelli and Springer-Verlag 2008
Abstract Several vertebrate species, including humans,
following passive spatial disorientation appear to be able to
reorient themselves by making use of the geometric shape of
the environment (i.e., metric properties of surfaces and
directional sense). In some circumstances, reliance on such
purely geometric information can overcome the use of local
featural cues (landmarks). The relative use of geometric and
non-geometric information seems to depend upon, among
other factors, the size of the experimental space. Evidence in
non-human animals and in human infants for primacy in
encoding either geometric or landmark information depend-
ing on the size of the environment is reviewed, together with
possible theoretical accounts of this phenomenon.
Keywords Spatial reorientation Geometry
Modularity Space size Human infants Chick
Pigeon Fish
Introduction
Among the wide range of spatial skills that allow animals
to navigate, the ability to reorient after disorientation in a
familiar environment can not be ignored. Spatial reorien-
tation plays an important role in natural conditions when an
animal has to re-establish the spatial relationship between
itself and its environment in order to find a particular
location (irrespective of whether a human-being is required
to return to a shop or to a restaurant or whether a rat has to
go back to its own hole; the underlying basic mechanisms
seem to be quite comparable (Vallortigara 2008). Reori-
entation is a basic adaptive strategy, strictly necessary to
figure out the exact relationship between an organism’s
current position and an already visited target location when
path integration is ruled out. How do organisms deal with
such spatial re-orientation problems?
Strategies and kinds of information
Two major and complementary coding strategies are in use
when an organism encodes the properties of an external
space: an egocentric- and an allocentric-based strategy.
The former is accountable for tracking the position of the
animal by encoding (and continuously updating) distances
and directions from the starting point during travel: it is
also known as path integration or dead reckoning. The
latter is responsible for mapping out locations by referring
to the characteristics of the surroundings, encoding spatial
relationships among cues, which can provide a stable frame
of reference with their relative positions since they do not
change as the animal moves. Each mechanism bears per se
informative properties and they can co-operate. In addition,
by doing so they may provide redundant information.
However, it may also occur, in some circumstances, that
the two systems are not in agreement, i.e., when a move-
ment not controlled by the animal itself displaces it (as
tumbling down a hill) or when there is a lack of external
C. Chiandetti (&)
Department of Psychology and B.R.A.I.N. Centre
for Neuroscience, University of Trieste,
Via S. Anastasio 12, 34123 Trieste, Italy
e-mail: cchiandetti@univ.trieste.it
URL: http://www.psico.univ.trieste.it/labs/acn-lab/eng_p/
e00_home.html
G. Vallortigara
Center for Mind/Brain Sciences, University of Trento,
Corso Bettini 31, 38068 Rovereto, Italy
e-mail: giorgio.vallortigara@unitn.it
123
Cogn Process (2008) 9:229–238
DOI 10.1007/s10339-008-0202-6
orienting cues (for instance in the dark). In those cases, the
internal system formed by inertial, kinaesthetic and ves-
tibular signals is inevitably disrupted and the objects’
relationships to the animal are no longer represented cor-
rectly but leave the external one still unaffected (since the
position and the orientation of the agent are not relevant for
correctly representing object-to-object relationships). Thus,
the allocentric-based mechanism is used to adjust the self-
based one and aspects of the external world must be used to
re-establish orientation as has been well-documented in a
wide variety of studies addressing this reorientation ability
(see Etienne and Jeffery 2004; Cheng 2005; Cheng and
Newcombe 2005 for exhaustive reviews).
To successfully orient and functionally navigate from
one place to another a detailed consideration of the overall
arrangement of spatial features available in the environment
is needed. Usually, at least in our experience as human-
beings, a place ‘can be objectively defined by both its
relationship to the environment and its intrinsic character-
istics (local cues)’ (Poucet 1993). In the natural
environment, namely the complete space surrounding an
individual (global level), local cues refer to all the distinc-
tive features that can be selectively taken into account, and
defined by relationships that link one single characteristic to
each other but that are still located in the ‘‘working space’’.
Of course, there are no physical boundaries that constrain a
local space with respect to a global one. However, because
of the particular indoor paradigm developed by K. Cheng to
study spatial reorientation abilities in animals (Cheng 1986)
the global space perceived by the animal is defined (and
confined) by the shape of the enclosure: i.e., the layout
composed of axes, surfaces and their relative lengths
together with their incident joint points. In the literature,
this geometric layout is referred to as geometric informa-
tion. Another source of information is supplied by the local
cues—non-geometric information in this paradigm—i.e.,
those salient properties of the surfaces such as their colours
and patterns or those features distinct from the surfaces
themselves such as separate beacons located in specific
positions. In natural environments, these two types of cue
are usually correlated although with varying degree from
one environment to another.
There is, then, another source of information that is
usually present while the animal is processing the sur-
roundings: the internal directional sense of left–right which
may be helpful in associating different cues with respect to
their specific and relative position in egocentric coordinates.
The original paradigm and the first results
A powerful experimental paradigm to study spatial reori-
entation abilities was first developed by K. Cheng in 1986.
In his study, rats (Rattus norvegicus) were located inside a
rectangular enclosure and were trained to find food,
available in one corner, in the presence of several visual
and olfactory cues. The correct location was defined by the
availability of both geometric properties of the surfaces
(i.e., shorter and longer walls together with the ‘‘sense’ for
left–right discrimination) and non-geometric properties
(visual and olfactory cues of the surfaces of the environ-
ment; a schematic representation of the task is shown in
Fig. 1). When the rats were reintroduced into the apparatus,
after having been passively disoriented outside in order to
Fig. 1 Top schematic representation of the geometrical information,
which is available in a rectangular-shaped environment. The target
(filled dot) stands in the same geometric relations to the shape of the
environment as its rotational equivalent (open dot). Metric informa-
tion (i.e., distinction between a short and a long wall) together with
sense (i.e., distinction between left and right) suffices to distinguish
between locations AC and locations BD, but not to distinguish
between A and C (or between B and D). Bottom schematic
representation of the non-geometrical information provided
by discrete panels placed in each corner. The target (filled dot)is
unambiguously defined by the presence of a specific and salient
visual cue
230 Cogn Process (2008) 9:229–238
123
remove inertial information (i.e., to avoid internal sense of
direction and location), they searched at an equal rate in the
target corner and in the corner located at 180° rotation from
the target (the indistinguishable opposite position with the
same geometrical properties with respect to the shape of
the enclosure).
Interestingly, rats did not resort to use of non-geometric
cues to solve the ambiguity of the room’s symmetry and
thus to discriminate between the two geometrically iden-
tical positions (A and C in Fig. 1). A combination of the
two sources of information, geometric and non-geometric,
would have led them to a correct choice but rats were able
to take into account both geometric and non-geometric
properties only after repeated trials (i.e., in a reference
memory task). However, even in reference memory tasks,
when the two kinds of information were placed in conflict
(using the so-called affine transformation, in which a dis-
location of the featural cues occurred so that the previously
reinforced cue was located in a novel, geometrically
incorrect position) rats preferred to choose the corners
located in the geometrically correct locations and with the
wrong features, confirming a primary role of geometric
information in reorientation.
Making use of the same paradigm as described for rats,
Hermer and Spelke in 1994 (and again in 1996) tested 18–
24-month-old children in a rectangular room (4 9 6-ft)
with either all four walls white (geometry-only condition)
or with three white walls and one blue wall (geometry plus
feature condition). In the reference memory task, toddlers
reoriented taking into consideration the geometric proper-
ties of the rectangular room in the white-wall testing
situation, but they continued to confuse systematically the
two geometrically equivalent locations in the blue wall
task, when featural information was available. Thus they
were demonstrated as having primarily used (as did rats)
the geometric shape of the environment to find the target
position. In contrast, human adults behaved in a completely
different manner being able to use geometry alone when in
the white-walls condition and to conjoin geometric and
non-geometric information to disentangle between geo-
metrically equivalent locations when tested in the blue wall
test (Hermer and Spelke 1994).
A modular architecture for spatial reorientation?
This pattern of data was accounted for by Hermer and
Spelke (1994) in terms of a modular architecture of cog-
nitive functions (Fodor 1983, 2001). The authors argued
that toddlers rely on an innate ‘geometric module’’, an
encapsulated and task-specific process, devoted to a subset
of information, i.e., purely geometrical information. The
evidence that older children (5–7 years old) appeared to be
able to disentangle the room’s symmetry when in the
presence of a blue wall (Hermer-Vasquez et al. 2001), led
the authors to suggest that the development of spatial
language (with use of ‘left’ and ‘right’ terms associated
with description of visual scenes) would be crucial for the
ability of integrating geometric and featural information
(Spelke 2003; Wang and Spelke 2002).
Further evidence for a role of language came from
studies in which human adults were required to solve the
spatial reorientation task while performing a verbal shad-
owing task so as to prevent use of verbal language.
Hermer-Vazquez et al. (1999) showed that, when tested
while performing a verbal shadowing task, human adults
displayed the same pattern of errors as children in the
presence of a blue wall, mirroring the inability of younger
infants to integrate geometric and non-geometric infor-
mation. When tested while performing a non-verbal
shadowing task (i.e., rhythm clapping), the same subjects
appeared able to conjoin successfully the two sources of
information, thus supporting the hypothesis of a role of
language to overcome the geometric module encapsulation
(but see Ratliff and Newcombe 2005; Hupbach and Nadel
2005; Newcombe 2005 for contrasting evidence).
However, although verbal language could be crucial for
the integration of geometric and non-geometric informa-
tion in humans (Carruthers 2002; Hermer-Vasquez et al.
1999), other species, devoid of linguistic capacities, can
nonetheless conjoin geometric and non-geometric infor-
mation in order to reorient themselves. Evidence has been
collected in this regard from a wide range of species,
including fish [redtail splitfins (Xenotoca eiseni): Sovrano
et al. 2002, 2003; goldfish (Carassius auratus): Vargas
et al. 2004], birds [domestic chicks (Gallus gallus): Val-
lortigara et al. 1990, 2004; pigeons (Columba livia): Kelly
et al. 1998) and mammals (rhesus monkeys (Macaca
mulatta): Gouteux et al. 2001; tamarins (Saguinus Oedi-
pus): Deipolyi et al. 2001].
Does size matter? Reorientation in enclosures
of different sizes
Learmonth et al. (2001) duplicated the original experi-
ments of Hermer and Spelke (1994) but found that young
infants successfully reoriented themselves by joining
together geometrical and featural information. However, an
uncontrolled variable across these two studies was the size
of the experimental room used. Evidence of an influence of
the environment size on navigation and on estimations of
distances had been previously noted in children (see
Herman and Siegel 1978; Siegel et al. 1979). Thus, in a
subsequent study, Learmonth et al. (2002) specifically
addressed this issue. They found that toddlers failed to
Cogn Process (2008) 9:229–238 231
123
consider environmental features as landmarks in order to
reorient themselves when tested in a small room (4 9 6-ft)
but, when tested in a larger room, double in size (8 9 12-
ft), the same children performed comparably to adults.
Hence, the spatial scale of the environment could be crucial
to the children’s ability to deal with the available sources
of information.
Human adults also displayed a similar pattern of
behaviour (Ratliff and Newcombe 2007): trained in either a
small or a large rectangular room with a feature landmark
along one of the walls and then tested after a displacement
of the visual cue only (i.e., while maintaining the room
constant in size), subjects rely on the geometric layout in
the small but not in the large room. In the larger environ-
ment they preferred to use the landmark information.
Moreover, when displaced from a small to a large room or
vice versa and again after the shift of the landmark, sub-
jects preferred to rely on the landmark even in the small
environment demonstrating evidence of an effect of expe-
rience in dealing with featural information.
Comparative studies on the effects of the size of the
enclosure on reorientation in non-human species revealed a
complex pattern of results (see Table 1 for a summary).
Sovrano et al. (2005) tested fish, redtail splitfins (Xenotoca
eiseni), both in a large (31 cm long 9 14 cm wide 9 16
cm high) and in a small (15 9 7 9 16 cm) tank with dis-
tinctively coloured walls that provided featural
information. Fish proved identically able to combine geo-
metric and featural information in both conditions;
however, when displaced from a large to a small tank, or
vice versa, from training to test, they tended to make rel-
atively more errors based on geometric information when
transfer occurred from a small to a large space, and to make
relatively more errors based on landmark information when
transfer occurred from a large to a small space.
Similarly to fish, domestic chicks (Gallus gallus) trained
in a large (70 cm long 9 35 cm wide 9 40 cm high) or in
a small (17.5 9 35 9 40 cm) enclosure showed no dif-
ferences in their reorientation ability, being capable in
successfully conjoining geometry and non-geometry in
either the large or the small arena. However, differently
from fish, chicks did not show any difference in the dis-
tribution of geometric and non-geometric errors when
displaced form the large to the small enclosure or vice
versa (Vallortigara et al. 2005). Only when the chicks were
tested after an affine transformation (i.e., an alteration of
the relations between different information that puts in
conflict geometric and non-geometric information) they
made more choices based on geometry when in the small
enclosure than when in the large enclosure, suggesting that
the use (or the primacy in the use) of geometric and non-
geometric information varies also in this species depending
on the size of the experimental space.
The thread running through all these species, from
humans to fish, seems to be that there is a general ten-
dency to rely more on geometry in smaller than in larger
environments. However, a qualification is needed: the
above mentioned results obtained with chicks refer to a
version of the paradigm in which non-geometrical cues
were supplied as discrete panels placed in correspondence
to every corner (as in the original test with rats), whereas
the children and fish were tested in the blue wall version
of the task (i.e., with one of the walls of the enclosure
made distinctive by use of a different colour). It could be
that in these circumstances chicks were representing space
on the basis of the arrangement of the discrete elements
themselves (the panels at the corners) and, if so, that the
different behaviour displayed in the large and in the small
enclosure was due to an encoding of the landmarks
(panels) at the target and of those close to it that happened
to be localized at different distances in large and small
enclosures. In other terms, it could be that the chicks
tended to use the distant panels to relocate the target
position and that this was more or less easy to do in a
small or in a large enclosure. In fact, if the chicks were
considering the target cue together with its nearest
neighbour along the short wall in the small space, after a
displacement to a larger arena they might consider the
target cue only, since the distances are doubled. On the
contrary, after being moved to a smaller arena, a new
panel appears close to the one located at the target posi-
tion. Hence, the affine transformation would change the
target feature in the small space but not in the large one.
Some recent work specifically addressed this issue.
Chiandetti et al. (2007) trained chicks to find food in a
corner of either a small or a large rectangular enclosure. A
distinctive panel was located at each of the four corners of
the enclosures. No differences in the encoding of the
overall arrangement of landmarks were apparent when
chicks were tested for generalization in an enclosure dif-
fering from that of training in size together with a
transformation (affine transformation) that altered the
geometric relations between the target and the shape of the
environment. The results were therefore not in accordance
with the hypothesis that the chicks encoded the target cue
and its nearest neighbour along the short wall in the small
enclosure but not in the large enclosure.
Further experiments tried to disentangle the relative
contribution of geometry and landmark cues in large and
small spaces (Chiandetti et al. 2007). Again, chicks were
trained to find food in a corner of either a small or a large
rectangular enclosure with distinctive panels located at
each of the four corners of the enclosures. After removal of
the panels, chicks tested in the small enclosure showed
better retention of geometrical information than chicks
tested in the large enclosure. In contrast, after changing the
232 Cogn Process (2008) 9:229–238
123
enclosure from a rectangular-shaped to a square-shaped
one while keeping the corner panels the same, chicks tested
in the large enclosure showed better retention of landmark
(panels) information than chicks tested in the small
enclosure. These findings strongly suggest that the primacy
of geometric or landmark information in reorientation tasks
depends on the size of the experimental space also in non-
human species.
Factors influencing a differential use of geometric and
non-geometric information in small and large spaces
A factor that could affect the probability of using non-
geometrical information in large spaces is the proportion of
the subject’s body size with respect to the experimental
space used (see Nadel and Hupbach 2006). In order to better
understand this point, the same animal should be tested at
Table 1 Summary of results coming from experiments addressing enclosures’ size effects on different species
Study Species Feature cue Space Test change Effect
Hermer and Spelke
(1994)
Human adults Blue wall Small Geometry + feature
18–24 months old
infants
Blue wall Small Geometry
2 Landmarks Small Geometry
Learmonth et al.
(2001)
17–24 months old
infants
2 Landmarks Large Feature
Landmark Large Geometry + feature
Blue wall Large Geometry + feature
Learmonth et al.
(2002)
3–4 years old children Blue wall Large or small Geometry + feature in
large
Geometry in small
5–6 years old children Blue wall Large or small Geometry + feature in
large
Geometry + feature in
small (younger children use
still geometry only in
small)
Ratliff and
Newcombe (2007)
Human adults Landmark Large or small Landmark shift Feature in large
Geometry in small
Landmark Large or small Size + landmark
shift
Feature in large and in
small (feature in small
more when trained in
large)
Vallortigara et al.
(2005)
Domestic chicks (Gallus
gallus)
Blue wall Large or small Size (from small to
large and
viceversa)
Good generalisation
Panels Large or small Affine
transformation
Feature in large
Feature + metric + sense
in small
Sovrano and
Vallortigara
(2006)
Domestic chicks (Gallus
gallus)
Blue wall Large or small Affine
transformation
Feature + sense in large
Metric + sense in small
Chiandetti et al.
(2007)
Domestic chicks (
Gallus
gallus)
Panels Large or small Size + affine
transformation
Feature
Removal of panels Geometry (more in small)
Removal of metric Feature (more in large)
Sovrano et al. (2005) Redtail splitfins
(Xenotoca eiseni)
Blue wall Large or small Size (from small to
large and vice
versa)
Good generalisation (more
geometric errors from
small to large; featural
errors from large to
small)
Sovrano et al. (2007) Redtail splitfins
(Xenotoca eiseni)
Blue wall Large or small Affine
transformation
Feature + sense in large
Metric + sense in small
(but also just metric)
Cogn Process (2008) 9:229–238 233
123
different developmental stages while maintaining the same
environmental size; of course, this is not possible since
developmental factors themselves could be sufficient to
explain differences (if any) acting like confounding vari-
ables. However, if we focus on the white-walls task,
different test enclosures were used either across species or
in the same species without affecting the performance. Rats,
for instance, were tested in a chamber that was 120 cm
long 9 60 cm wide (Cheng 1986); domestic chicks (not
much bigger than rats at the age tested) performed the task
in an enclosure of similar size (Vallortigara et al. 1990)as
well as in a smaller one, sized 70 cm long 9 35 cm wide
(Vallortigara et al. 2004; Chiandetti et al. 2007). Hence, no
differences were observed in manipulating geometry.
A second notable element is the size of the featural cue
itself. It may play a crucial role in reorientation perfor-
mance, especially when this non-geometrical information
is provided by a coloured wall, the absolute size of which
changes accordingly in a small or in a large room (Shus-
terman and Spelke 2005). It cannot be ruled out that, when
perceived as being too close by the subject, a coloured wall
may be not properly considered and therefore not pro-
cessed as a cue. Nadel and Hupbach (2006) claimed that
animals prefer to rely upon distal rather than proximal cues
as landmarks for navigation (see also Gallistel 1990). From
this point of view, in small spaces where featural cues end
up located close to the subject, non-geometric information
could be treated as proximal information; in larger spaces,
in contrast, featural cues could be perceived as far-located,
thus increasing the probability for them to be used as distal
information.
This makes sense ecologically since, when navigating in
natural environments, it is more reliable to rely on distal
salient characteristics that do not disappear quickly rather
than on local proximal cues, which are subject to changes
in their relative position while the animal moves in the
environment (Parron et al. 2004). Hence, there may be a
sort of predisposition to treat non-geometric features as
orienting cues (landmarks) only when they are perceived as
far away, but not when they are in close proximity (Nadel
and Hupbach 2006). It is an interesting question in this
regard whether ‘absolute size’ of the wall is important, or
rather if proximity to the subject affects the evaluation,
since in the usual setup a larger room has a wall that is both
greater in absolute size and further away from the subject,
these factors are not the same (and in fact could be
experimentally dissociated).
Recently, Learmonth et al. (2008) tested children of
different ages (from 3 to 6 years old) in various conditions
with the aim of clarifying the role of distal and proximal
landmark use in environments of different sizes. The room
was a rectangular 8 9 11-ft space with one coloured wall;
an inner enclosure sized 4 9 6-ft and 18 inches high was
located in the centre of the room. When the target was
located in the inner enclosure, only 6-year old children
found the correct location whereas younger children relied
only on geometry (confirming previous findings on the
ability of older children to manage correctly all the avail-
able information). When the target was hidden in the outer
room, with the children still confined in the inner rectan-
gular space, 4- as well as 5-year-old infants proved able to
combine geometry and non-geometry to find the goal
object whlist 3-year-olds again chose either of the two
congruent locations (Learmonth et al., in press). This pat-
tern of results is likely to be explained on the basis of a
developmental progression in the use of proximal cues as
informative ones.
Data on the importance of distal cues also come from
neurobiological experiments (Zugaro et al. 2004). Rats
underwent tests in an enclosure in which orienting cues
were provided by the presence of a foreground (proximal)
and a background (distal) card. Electrophysiological
recordings were collected under either continuous light or
stroboscopic light, a condition in which, by disturbing
some dynamic visual processes (such as motion parallax
and optic flow), rats were unable to refer to distal cards.
Responses of head-direction cells appeared to be dependent
on the information conveyed by distal cues, which are
more stable when the animal moves about.
The hypothesis that organisms are prepared to use only
distant featural information as landmarks for reorientation
(Wang and Spelke 2002; Spelke 2003; Hupbach and Nadel
2005) meets, however, with difficulties. One problem with
this view is that, given the evidence of a primacy of geo-
metric information over non-geometric information (see
Cheng and Newcombe 2005 for a review), the basic issue is
not to explain why organisms do not use featural infor-
mation in small spaces (they could do that simply because
of the primacy of geometric information), but rather to
explain why they do not continue to use geometric infor-
mation even when tested in large spaces. This is
particularly intriguing, because it has been usually main-
tained that large-scale geometric information may provide
more stable and reliable cues than local environmental
features such as landmarks (Cheng and Newcombe 2005).
Moreover, species-specific differences in dealing with
the overall sources of information should be considered.
Conceivably, reorienting making use simultaneously of
metric (long–short), features (white–blue) and sense (left–
right) information could be costly: it may become difficult
to handle all this information together, at least in non-
human species, and therefore some of them could be sep-
arately considered with a preserved possibility to reorient
successfully.
Sovrano and Vallortigara (2006) produced evidence
suggesting that sense for left–right distinction could be
234 Cogn Process (2008) 9:229–238
123
separately associated with geometry and landmark infor-
mation depending on the spatial scale of the environment.
Consider the blue wall version of the task, and the condi-
tion in which the correct position (faced by the chick, in
Fig. 2) is properly defined by having, while facing it, a blue
short wall on the right and a long white wall on the left.
The encoding process that could be used here may rely
either only on sense (left–right) and metric (long–short)
thus having a definition like ‘‘a short wall on the right and a
long wall on the left’’, or only on sense and the non-geo-
metric cue thus having a definition like ‘a blue wall on the
right and a white wall on the left’’.
When chicks were trained in either a large or a small
enclosure and then tested in enclosures of the same size as
used during training, after a blue wall dislocation (e.g.,
with the blue wall on the short wall during training and on
the long wall at test) they showed a different linkage of
sense information with either metric or landmark infor-
mation depending on the spatial scale of the environment.
That is, in small spaces chicks linked sense with metric
properties of surfaces, in large spaces they linked sense
with local landmark cues (Sovrano and Vallortigara 2006;
and see also Sovrano et al. 2007 for similar results with
fish).
This is shown in Fig. 2. Assuming that visual analysis
of a corner (for instance by head-direction cells) occurs at
a fixed distance from the animal, then in a small envi-
ronment (Fig. 2, right) the available information provided
by complete scanning of the length of the surfaces may
provide a reliable source of information for spatial reori-
entation. Thus, animals may rely on an association
between the metric properties of the surfaces and the sense
(the correct corner has a short wall on the right and a long
wall on the left). In a large environment, however, scan-
ning of the full spatial extent of the surfaces is prevented
(Fig. 2, left). Thus, the animals must rely on an associa-
tion between the featural properties of the surfaces and the
sense (the correct corner has a blue feature on the right
and a white feature on the left). According to Sovrano and
Vallortigara (2006) such different associations depend on
what the animal is pondering in environments of different
spatial scale.
This hypothesis is not necessarily in contrast with claims
for a preference of using distal rather than proximal cues
for reorientation, but provides a more precise account of
the overall pattern of findings with the rectangular task in
large and small enclosures in a variety of species.
Further support for this view comes from results
obtained with people with Williams syndrome, a genetic
deficit that severely compromises spatial representations
while relatively preserving language (e.g. Brown et al.
2003): these patients are good performers when tested in
the blue wall task thus proving able to conjoin a metric
property with the left–right sense whereas they fail in the
white-wall version, i.e., fail in using geometry alone
(Landau et al. 2006). Obviously this would imply that in
the blue wall task these subjects would perform differently
when the target was located between two same-colour
corners (in which only metric information can be used) or
between two differently coloured corners (in which colour
plus sense can be used as an alternative to use of metric
plus sense). No evidence for such a difference is currently
available. Interestingly, however, Nardini et al. (2008)
reported recently that children of 18–24 months old can
reorient using the left–right sense of coloured landmarks
(and see also Burgess 2006).
Concluding remarks
Many different species have been shown to be able to
reorient themselves encoding and remembering the geo-
metrical information provided by the metric arrangements
of the walls of a rectangular enclosure together with
available non-geometric cues. However, differences in the
relative reliance on geometric and non-geometric cues have
been observed when animals were tested in enclosures of
different sizes.
The results discussed in this review bring forward evi-
dence that animals prefer to use geometric information
when in a small environment and prefer to rely on land-
mark cues when in a large environment.
Fig. 2 Photograph layout of encoding occurring in a large (left) and a
small (right) rectangular enclosures with a blue wall
Cogn Process (2008) 9:229–238 235
123
Given that there is substantial agreement in arguing for a
prominent role of geometry in natural environments (see
Introduction) due to its stable and invariant characteristics
across time, it is not difficult to believe that vertebrates are
somehow innately predisposed to manage geometry for
successful navigation (Chiandetti and Vallortigara 2008;
Vallortigara and Sovrano 2002). Hence, a hierarchical way
to process different kinds of information resembles the
hierarchical structure of the environment.
When we think of a natural environment, it is not dif-
ficult to note that usually several redundant cues are
available and that if one organism simply relies on just one
at a time the strategy could prove successful. Such a pattern
of reliance (one at a time) may directly reflect the path of
evolution (Simon 1969) with an addition of new modules
rather than a change in the old ones (Shettleworth 1998).
In order to unravel how they are weighted, different cues
can be placed in conflict so that one cue indicates one goal
location and the other cue another goal location. The rel-
ative weightings of different sources of information may
change with different conditions and it can happen that the
animal resorts to use one whilst disregarding another. Thus,
some cues may obtain a primacy in processing and hence
they may provide a context for subsequent analyses (see
also Shettleworth 1998).
However, the unresolved question is, why do animals
resort to using geometry in a small environment but prefer
landmarks in larger ones. A challenging hypothesis comes
from the possibility that there is a different association
between metric, sense and landmarks cues as suggested by
Sovrano and Vallortigara (2006). When located in a small
enclosure an organism has the metric layout of the surfaces
around it available to it, thus adding the sense for left–right
discrimination it has a reliable source of information for
spatial reorientation. When an organism is in a larger
enclosure, estimation of the lengths of extended surfaces
would require costly scanning or direct movements back
and forward: this could lead it to prefer to associate a
featural property immediately available in the vicinity with
the left–right sense, thus discarding use of geometry.
Such different associations could be expected since they
convey more reliable information to an organism on the
basis of visual (or other sensory) scanning of environments
of different spatial scale.
However, what does reliable really mean here with
respect to size? If we assume that reliability in large as well
as small environments is directly linked to the source
of information that conveys the most information for the
least effort (i.e., efficient information) and it is also the
most stable, the type of information on which there is a
reliance can change according to the size of the environ-
ment. In a small environment things change rapidly as the
animal moves; in this case, geometry is the most stable
information whereas local information is difficult to com-
pute. In a large environment, the features are less likely to
change: while moving, they require a long time to disap-
pear from the visual field. In this sense, landmark cues are
stable and informative. Now, it is useful to distinguish
between directional and positional information. All distal
information allows great efficiency in guiding navigation
because—as discussed in the previous sections—it pro-
vides stable directional and useful positional information.
Proximal cues may have more precise positional informa-
tion but offer a much reduced directional information.
Moreover, motion parallax effects and large changes in
visual signals are high for proximal landmarks, therefore it
would probably be simpler relying on distal landmarks in a
large environment and it would probably be less difficult
using metric differences in a small environment, in the
sense that each kind of information acquires different
values of predictive strength as a function of their
usefulness.
Thus, an alternative explanation can be put forward on
the basis that two different mechanisms are involved, one
for distal and one for proximal cues’ use, as suggested from
neurobiological works, at least with respect to rats (Parron
et al. 2004). Specifically, it has been shown that entorhinal
cortex lesioned rats were compromised in using distal
landmarks but not proximal ones (Parron et al. 2004). Of
course, the role of the entorhinal cortex in navigation is far
from being clear, and it is not the aim of this review to
discuss it. However, the authors suggest that enthorinal-
hippocampal circuitry processes the distal cues whereas the
parietal-hippocampal circuitry accounts for proximal cues.
Moreover, single unit recording studies on place cells
(Cressant et al. 1997) as well as on direction cells (Zugaro
et al. 2001) seem to confirm a dissociation between distal-
proximal landmark processing.
What exactly happens while an animal is reorienting in
environments of different spatial scale is of course still
matter of debate (see Lee et al. 2006). The two alternative
views suggested in this review are not in contrast. From the
behavioural side, there should be another (unverified)
condition replicating Chiandetti et al. (2007) condition
(with both the change in size and the affine transformation
from training to test) but with the blue wall; in this con-
dition it should be expected a stronger effect on different
linkage among separate kind of information on the basis of
the experimental space size used. From the neural side,
data coming from neurobiological studies will maybe bring
some clarifications on the way different sources of infor-
mation are encoded and processed before the overt choice
of the organism.
Acknowledgments This work was supported by grants MIUR Cofin
2004, 2004070353_002 ‘Intellat’ and MIPAF ‘Benolat’ (to GV).
236 Cogn Process (2008) 9:229–238
123
We would like to thank two anonymous referees for their suggestions
on a previous version of this manuscript.
References
Burgess N (2006) Spatial memory: how egocentric and allocentrico
combine. Trends Cogn Sci 10:551–557
Brown JH, Johnson MH, Peterson SJ, Gilmore R, Longhi E,
Karmiloff-Smith A (2003) Spatial representation and attention
in toddlers with Williams syndrome and Down syndrome.
Neuropsychologia 41:1037–1046
Carruthers P (2002) The cognitive functions of language. Behav Brain
Sci 25:657–726
Cheng K (1986) A purely geometric module in the rat’s spatial
representation. Cognition 23:149–178
Cheng K (2005) Reflections on geometry and navigation. Conn Sci
17:5–21
Cheng K, Newcombe NS (2005) Is there a geometric module for
spatial orientation? Squaring theory and evidence. Psychon Bull
Rev 12:1–23
Chiandetti C, Regolin L, Sovrano VA, Vallortigara G (2007) Spatial
reorientation: the effects of space size on the encoding of
landmark and geometry information. Anim Cogn 10:159–168
Chiandetti C, Vallortigara G (2008) Is there an innate geometric
module? Effects of experience with angular geometric cues on
spatial re-orientation based on the shape of the environment.
Anim Cogn 11:139–146
Cressant A, Muller RU, Poucet B (1997) Failure of centrally placed
objects to control the firing fields of hippocampal place cells. J
Neurosci 17:2531–42
Deipolyi A, Santos L, Hauser MD (2001) The role of landmarks in
cotton-top tamarin spatial foraging: Evidence for geometric and
non-geometric features. Anim Cogn 4:99–108
Etienne AS, Jeffery KJ (2004) Path integration in mammals.
Hippocampus 14:180–192
Fodor J (1983) The modularity of mind. MIT Press, Cambridge
Fodor J (2001) The mind doesn’t work that way. MIT Press,
Cambridge
Gallistel CR (1990) The Organization of Learning. MIT Press,
Cambridge
Gouteux S, Thinus-Blanc C, Vauclair J (2001) Rhesus monkeys use
geometric and nongeometric information during a reorientation
task. J Exp Psych Gen 130:505–519
Herman JF, Siegel AW (1978) The development of cognitive mapping
of the large scale environment. J Exp Child Psych 26:389–406
Hermer L, Spelke ES (1994) A geometric process for spatial
reorientation in young children. Nature 370:57–59
Hermer L, Spelke ES (1996) Modularity and development: The case
of spatial reorientation. Cognition 61:195–232
Hermer-Vasquez L, Spelke ES, Katsnelson AS (1999) Sources of
flexibility in human cognition: dual-task studies of space and
language. Cogn Psych 39: 3–36
Hermer-Vasquez L, Moffet A, Munkholm P (2001) Language, space,
and the development of cognitive flexibility in humans: the case
of two spatial memory tasks. Cognition 78:263–299
Hupbach A, Nadel L (2005) Reorientation in a rhombic environment:
no evidence for an encapsulated geometric module. Cogn
Develop 20:279–302
Kelly DM, Spetch ML, Heth CD (1998) Pigeons (Columba livia)
encoding of geometric and featural properties of a spatial
environment. J Comp Psych 112:259–269
Landau B, Lakusta L, Dessalegn B (2006) Failure to represent
geometry in people with Williams Syndrome? Abstracts of the
47th Annual Meeting if the Psychonomic Society
Learmonth AE, Nadel L, Newcombe NS (2002) Children’s use of
landmarks: Implication for modularity theory. Psych Sci 13:337–
341
Learmonth AE, Newcombe NS, Huttenlocher J (2001) Toddlers’ use
of metric information and landmarks to reorient. J Exp Child
Psych 80:225–244
Learmonth AE, Newcombe NS, Sheridan N, Jones M (2008) Why
size counts: children’s spatial reorientation in large and small
enclosures (in press)
Lee SA, Shusterman A, Spelke E (2006) Reorientation and landmark-
guided search by young children. Evidence for two systems.
Psych Sci 17:577–582
Nadel L, Hupbach A (2006) Spacies comparisons in development: the
case of the spatial ‘module’’. In: Johnson M, Munakata Y (eds)
Processes of change in brain and cognitive development.
Attention and Performance XXI, Oxford University Press,
Oxford
Nardini M, Atkinson J, Burgess N (2008) Children reorient using the
left/right sense of coloured landmarks at 18–24 months. Cogni-
tion 106:519–527
Newcombe NS (2005) Evidence for and against a geometric module:
the roles of language and action. In: Rieser J, Lockman J, Nelson
C (eds) Action as an organizer of learning and development.
Minnesota symposium on child psychology. Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates, Mahwah, pp 221–241
Parron C, Poucet B, Save E (2004) Entorhinal cortex lesions impair
the use of distal but not proximal landmarks during place
navigation in the rat. Behav Brain Res 154:345–352
Poucet B (1993) Spatial cognitive maps in animals: new hypotheses
on their structure and neural mechanisms. Psy Rev 100:163–182
Ratliff KR, Newcombe NS (2005) Human spatial reorientation using
dual task paradigms. In: Proceedings of the 27th annual meeting
of the Cognitive Science Society
Ratliff KR, Newcombe NS (2007) A matter of trust: when landmarks
and geometry are used during reorientation. In: McNamara DS,
Trafton JG (eds) Proceedings of the 29th Annual Cognitive
Science Society. Cognitive Science Society, Austin, p 581
Siegel AW, Herman JF, Allen GL, Kirasic KC (1979) The develop-
ment of cognitive maps of large and small scale space. Child
Dev 50:582–585
Simon HA (1969) The sciences of the artificial. MIT Press, Cambridge
Shettleworth SJ (1998) Cognition, evolution and Behaviour. Oxford
Univeristy Press, New York
Shusterman A, Spelke ES (2005) Language and the development of
spatial reasoning. In: Carruthers P, Laurence S, Stitch S (eds)
The innate mind: structure and content. Oxford University Press,
Oxford
Sovrano VA, Vallortigara G (2006) Dissecting the geometric module:
a sense-linkage for metric and landmark information in animals’
spatial reorientation. Psych Sci 17:616–621
Sovrano VA, Bisazza A, Vallortigara G (2002) Modularity and spatial
reorientation in a simple mind: encoding of geometric and
nongeometric properties of a spatial environment by fish.
Cognition 85:B51–B59
Sovrano VA, Bisazza A, Vallortigara G (2003) Modularity as a fish
views it: conjoining geometric and nongeometric information for
spatial reorientation. J Exp Psych Anim Behav Proc 29:199–210
Sovrano VA, Bisazza A, Vallortigara G (2005) Animals’ use of
landmarks and metric information to reorient: effects of the size
of the experimental space. Cognition 97:121–133
Sovrano VA, Bisazza A, Vallortigara G (2007) How fish do geometry
in large and in small spaces. Anim Cogn 10:47–54
Spelke ES (2003) What makes us smart. Core knowledge and natural
language. In: Genter D, Goldin-Meadow S (eds) Language in
mind. Advances in the study of language and thought. MIT
Press, Cambridge, pp 277–311
Cogn Process (2008) 9:229–238 237
123
Vallortigara G (2006) The cognitive chicken: visual and spatial
cognition in a non-mammalian brain. In: Wasserman EA, Zentall
TR (eds) Comparative cognition: experimental explorations of
animal intelligence. Oxford University Press, Oxford
Vallortigara G (2008) Animals as natural geometers. In: Tommasi L,
Peterson M, Nadel L (eds) The biology of cognition. MIT Press,
Cambridge (in press)
Vallortigara G, Sovrano VA (2002) Conjoining information from
different modules: a comparative perspective. Behav Brain Sci
25:701–702
Vallortigara G, Feruglio M, Sovrano VA (2005) Reorientation by
geometric and landmark information in environments of differ-
ent spatial size. Devel Sci 8:393–401
Vallortigara G, Zanforlin M, Pasti G (1990) Geometric modules in
animal’s spatial representation: a test with chicks. J Comp Psych
104:248–254
Vallortigara G, Pagni P, Sovrano VA (2004) Separate geometric and
non-geometric modules for spatial reorientation: Evidence from
a lopsided animal brain. J Cogn Neurosci 16:390–400
Vargas JP, Lopez JC, Salas C, Thinus-Blanc C (2004) Encoding of
geometric and featural spatial information by Goldfish (Caras-
sius auratus). J Comp Psych 118:206–216
Wang RF, Spelke ES (2002) Human spatial representation: Insights
from animals. Trends Cogn Sci 6:376–382
Zugaro MB, Arleo A, Dejean C, Burguiere E, Khamassi M, Wiener SI
(2004) Rat anterodorsal thalamic head direction neurons depend
upon dynamic visual signals to select anchoring landmark cues.
Eur J Neurosci 20:530–536
Zugaro MB, Berthoz A, Wiener SI (2001) Background, but not
foreground, spatial cues are taken as references for head direction
responses by rat anterodorsal thalamus neurons. J Neurosci 21:
RC154
238 Cogn Process (2008) 9:229–238
123
... Interestingly, preference for the feature cue over the geometric cue appears to depend on the size of the environment. In general, evidence suggests that geometric cues are preferred in small enclosures and feature cues are preferred in large enclosures (Chiandetti, Regolin, Sovrano, & Vallortigara, 2007;Learmonth, Nadel, & Newcombe, 2002;Sovrano, Bisazza, & Vallortigara, 2007;Sovrano, Bisazza, & Vallortigara, 2005;Vallortigara, Feruglio, & Sovrano, 2005; see also , Miller, 2009; for a review, see Chiandetti & Vallortigara, 2008). ...
... There is some debate concerning the source(s) of these enclosuresize effects on relative spatial cue use. For example, Chiandetti and Vallortigara (2008) have suggested that geometric cues are easier to apprehend in small environments as opposed to large environ-ments. Given that geometric cues must be extracted from the overall shape of the enclosure, small environments may provide an opportunity to view multiple surfaces of an enclosure simultaneously which might facilitate the ability to discern the overall enclosure shape; in contrast, large environments necessarily prevent an opportunity to view multiple enclosure surfaces simultaneously which might inhibit the ability to discern the overall Figure 1. ...
Article
Full-text available
During spatial reorientation, the use of local geometric cues (e.g., corner angles) and global geometric cues (e.g., principal axis) is differentially influenced by enclosure size. Local geometric cues exert more influence in large enclosures compared to small enclosures, whereas the use of global geometric cues is not influenced by changes in enclosure size. Such effects are suggested to occur because of differences in training enclosures sizes or differences in testing enclosure sizes, but investigations of enclosure-size effects on spatial cue use have been confounded by environmental scaling between training and testing. We trained participants in a trapezoid-shaped enclosure to respond to a corner uniquely specified by both local and global geometric cues and tested participants in a rectangle (isolating the use of global geometric cues) and in a parallelogram (placing local and global geometric cues in conflict). Between groups, participants experienced different training environment sizes but identical testing environment sizes or identical training environment sizes but different testing environment sizes, and this allowed categorization with respect to the direction of environmental scaling. We found that environmental scaling between training and testing size (but not training size differences or testing size differences) influenced the relative use of local geometric cues. The use of global geometric cues was not influenced by enclosure size. Results challenge prior explanations of the influence of enclosure size on relative spatial cue use during spatial reorientation.
... 1. Development. Controlled-rearing experiments with chicks, mice, and fish have shown that reorientation by geometry does not depend on experience with layouts having a particular geometric structure (Brown et al., 2007;Chiandetti and Vallortigara, 2008;Twyman et al., 2013). ...
... In the smaller space, children did not use the featural information, while in the larger space those same children used both featural and geometric information. An effect of size on encoding of features and geometry has also been observed in both chicks (Vallortigara et al., 2005;Chiandetti et al., 2007;Chiandetti and Vallortigara, 2008) and fish . The common finding is that bias to rely on geometry for spatial reorientation over features tends to decrease as environment size increases (Chiandetti et al., 2007). ...
Thesis
To interact rapidly and effectively with the environment, the mammalian brain needs a representation of the spatial layout of the external world (or a “cognitive map”). A person might need to know where she is standing to find her way home, for instance, or might need to know where she is looking to reach for her out-of-sight keys. For many behaviors, however, simply possessing a map is not enough; in order for a map to be useful in a dynamic world, it must be anchored to stable environmental cues. The goal of the present research is to address this spatial anchoring problem in two different domains: navigation and vision. In the first part of the thesis, which comprises Chapters 1-3, we examine how navigators use perceptual information to re-anchor their cognitive map after becoming lost, a process known as spatial reorientation. Using a novel behavioral paradigm with rodents, in Chapter 2 we show that the cognitive map is reoriented by dissociable inputs for identifying where one is and recovering which way one is facing. The findings presented in Chapter 2 also highlight the importance of environmental boundaries, such as the walls of a room, for anchoring the cognitive map. We thus predicted that there might exist a brain region that is selectively involved in boundary perception during navigation. Accordingly, in Chapter 3, we combine transcranial magnetic stimulation and virtual-reality navigation to reveal the existence of such a boundary perception region in humans. In the second part of this thesis, Chapter 4, we explore whether the same mechanisms that support the cognitive map of navigational space also mediate a map of visual space (i.e., where one is looking). Using functional magnetic resonance imaging and eye tracking, we show that human entorhinal cortex supports a map-like representation of visual space that obeys the same principles of boundary-anchoring previously observed in rodent maps of navigational space. Together, this research elucidates how mental maps are anchored to the world, thus allowing the mammalian brain to form durable spatial representations across body and eye movements.
... Learmonth, Nadel, and Newcombe ( 2002 ) found that young children could use featural information to disambiguate geometry when tested in a large room but not when tested in a small room. An eff ect of size on encoding of features and geometry has since been found in both domestic chicks (Chiandetti, Regolin, Sovrano, & Vallortigara, 2007 ;Chiandetti & Vallortigara, 2008 ;Vallortigara, Feruglio, & Sovrano, 2005 ) and in fi sh (Sovrano, Bisazza, & Vallortigara, 2007 ). Th e common fi nding is that reliance on geometry over features tends to decrease as environment size increases. ...
... Th us, this species does not appear to require exposure to a geometrically rich environment in order to use geometry to orient, but rearing condition altered the weight given to geometry and features. Chiandetti and Vallortigara ( 2008 ) conducted a similar rearing study with a highly precocial species, the domestic chick. Th e chicks were dark incubated and then were raised individually in enclosed rectangular, circular, or C-shaped cages. ...
Article
This chapter provides a selective discussion of some main themes in the comparative investigation of geometric encoding for spatial navigation, and is meant to complement rather than duplicate other recent reviews. Although most investigations of geometric encoding have concerned the use of geometric shape formed by surfaces for reorientation, this chapter discusses the encoding of geometric information in a broader range of situations, including when participants are not disoriented and when spatial information is derived from arrays of landmarks.
... Testing for spatial cognition can also be affected by physical limitations, in that the relative size of the animal to the maze can affect perceptual boundaries and subsequent navigational strategies (Jacobs & Menzel, 2014). Several studies have found that navigational strategies may change depending on the size of the testing enclosure (reviewed in Chiandetti & Vallortigara, 2008). For example, mice tested on a larger Barnes maze used spatial cues during recall, whereas mice on a smaller Barnes maze used a serial search strategy, resulting in poorer performance on the spatial probe test (O'Leary & Brown, 2012). ...
Article
Full-text available
Probing for spatial cognitive processes in model rodent species has a long history in the psychological literature, with well-established protocols and paradigms successfully revealing the mechanisms underlying spatial learning and memory. There has also been much interest in examining the ecological and evolutionary context of spatial cognition, with a focus on how selection has molded spatial cognitive abilities in nonmodel species, how spatial cognitive traits vary across species, the neural mechanisms underlying spatial cognitive abilities, and the fitness outcomes of spatial cognition. Behavioral ecologists have been able to take advantage of paradigms from experimental psychology's rich history of spatial cognitive testing for use in nonmodel species. However, as the field advances, it is important to highlight noncognitive factors that can impact performance on spatial cognitive tasks (e.g., motivation to perform the task, switching navigational strategies, variation across protocols, ecological relevance of the task), as these factors may explain discrepancies in findings among some studies. This review highlights how these noncognitive factors can differentially modulate performance on spatial cognitive tests in different nonmodel species. Accounting for these factors when creating protocols and paradigms allows for a more nuanced approach with more explanatory power when probing for spatial cognitive abilities in nonmodel species. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).
... Alternatively, it might simply have been unnecessary to depend on something like a beacon in a small area such as the one we used, and this might have resulted in the strong choice of the original goal. In tests of geometric cues using larger, rectangular arenas, nongeometric, feature information such as wall color or discrete landmarks are preferentially used (Chiandetti & Vallortigara, 2008;Maes et al., 2009). Further work should investigate whether hamsters uses different types of cue information according to the topography and area in larger arenas; conceivably, hamsters might then depend on beacon-like features, as do other species. ...
Article
Full-text available
We investigated if and how Syrian hamsters (Mesocricetus auratus) show flexibility in their use of multiple sources of information in a spatial learning task. In Experiment 1, hamsters were trained to exit an arena through one of three doors. The goal, marked by a beacon, was fixed during the training phase. When the beacon was placed nearer to another door, the hamsters predominantly chose the original door rather than those with the relocated beacon (Tests A and C). The removal of the beacon had little effect on their performance (Test B). In Experiment 2, when we changed the shape of the apparatus to make the positional information of the goals other than the beacon invalid, a few individual subjects used the beacon (Test B'). However, after experiencing the invalidity of such positional information, the original door was less frequently chosen in a rerun of the beacon relocation test (Test C). Finally, in Experiment 3, we confirmed that hamsters could easily learn to use the beacon when it was the sole cue for navigation. These results suggest that, despite hamsters' limited tendency to use a beacon for navigation, they can flexibly use multiple strategies depending on their experience or environmental context. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).
... In addition, the overall latency to reach the platform and the latency to first entry the zone "A," where the platform was located, during training were not statistically different among the groups (SI Appendix, Fig. S5 C and D). Nevertheless, the test revealed some remarkable differences concerning the use of boundary-based versus landmark conditions in spatial navigation (51,55,56). In particular, both in landmark and boundary-based conditions, the wild-type animals spent significantly more time in the zone "A" (platform) compared to both heterozygous and homozygous mutants (Fig. 4 D and E and SI Appendix, Fig. S5E), suggesting that wild-type mice developed proper spatial reference information within the environment while mutants maintained a certain level of uncertainty. ...
Article
Full-text available
Significance Generation of new neurons occurs normally in the adult brain in two locations: the subventricular zone (SVZ) in the walls of the lateral ventricles and the subgranular zone (SGZ) in the dentate gyrus (DG) of the hippocampus. Neurogenesis in the adult hippocampus has been implicated in cognitive functions such as learning, memory, and recovery of stress response. Imprinted genes are highly prevalent in the brain and have adult and developmental important functions. Genetic deletion of the imprinted gene Dlk1 from either parental allele shows that DLK1 is a key mediator of quiescence in adult hippocampal NSCs. Additionally, Dlk1 is exquisitely dosage sensitive in the brain with perturbations in levels resulting in impaired NSCs function and cognitive phenotypes.
... Children were escorted into an 8' × 11' space with a 4' × 6' × 18″ inner enclosure ( Figure 1a). The inner barrier was used to confine children's movement to a smaller range, which has been shown to increase the difficulty of the task across species (for a review, see Chiandetti & Vallortigara, 2008), thus adding individual variability and reducing the potential for ceiling effects in children's performance. The walls of the larger space were defined by curtains, and the room's ceiling was formed by navy fabric clipped to wires on opposite sides of the room so that no external ceiling or lighting cues were provided, although the external lights were turned off during testing. ...
Article
The hippocampus is a subcortical structure in the medial temporal lobe involved in cognitive functions such as spatial navigation and reorientation, episodic memory, and associative learning. While much is understood about the role of hippocampal function in learning and memory in adults, less is known about the relations between the hippocampus and the development of these cognitive skills in young children due to the limitations of using standard methods (e.g., MRI) to examine brain structure and function in developing populations. The current study used hippocampal‐dependent trace eyeblink conditioning (EBC) as a feasible approach to examine individual differences in hippocampal functioning as they relate to spatial reorientation and episodic memory performance in young children. Three‐ to six‐year‐old children (N=50) completed tasks that measured EBC, spatial reorientation, and episodic memory, as well as non‐hippocampal‐dependent processing speed abilities. Results revealed that when age was held constant, individual differences in EBC performance were significantly related to individual differences in performance on the spatial reorientation test, but not on the episodic memory or processing speed tests. When the relations between hippocampal‐dependent EBC and different reorientation strategies were explored, it was found that individual differences in hippocampal function predicted the use of geometric information for reorienting in space as opposed to a combined strategy that uses both geometric information and salient visual cues. The utilization of eyeblink conditioning to examine hippocampal function in young populations and its implications for understanding the dissociation between spatial reorientation and episodic memory development are discussed. This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.
Article
Boundaries are crucial to our representation of the geometric shape of scenes, which can be used to reorient in space. Behavioral research has shown that children and adults share exquisite sensitivity to a defining feature of a boundary: its vertical extent. Imaging studies have shown that this boundary property is represented in the parahippocampal place area (PPA) among typically developed (TD) adults. Here, we show that sensitivity to the vertical extent of scene boundaries is impaired at both the behavioral and neural level in people with Williams syndrome (WS), a genetic deficit that results in severely impaired spatial functions. Behavioral reorientation was tested in three boundary conditions: a flat Mat, a 5 cm high Curb, and full Walls. Adults with WS could reorient in a rectangular space defined by Wall boundaries, but not Curb or Mat boundaries. In contrast, TD age-matched controls could reorient by all three boundary types and TD 4-year-olds could reorient by either Wall or Curb boundaries. Using fMRI, we find that the WS behavioral deficit is echoed in their neural representation of boundaries. While TD age-matched controls showed distinct neural responses to scenes depicting Mat, Curb, and Wall boundaries in the PPA, people with WS showed only a distinction between the Wall and Mat or Curb, but no distinction between the Mat and Curb. Taken together, these results reveal a close coupling between the representation of boundaries as they are used in behavioral reorientation and neural encoding, suggesting that damage to this key element of spatial representation may have a genetic foundation.
Article
Full-text available
Three experiments investigated how the room size affects preferential use of geometric and non-geometric cues during reorientation inside a room. We hypothesized that room size may affect preferential use of geometric and non-geometric cues by affecting the encoding of the cues (the encoding hypothesis), the retrieval of the cues (the retrieval hypothesis), or both the encoding and retrieval of the cues (the encoding-plus-retrieval hypothesis). In immersive virtual rectangular rooms, participants learned objects’ locations with respect to geometric (room shape) and non-geometric cues (features on walls or isolated objects). During the test, participants localized objects with the geometric cue only, non-geometric cues only, or both. The two cues were placed at the original locations or displaced relative to each other (conflicting cues) when both were presented at testing. We manipulated the room size between participants within each experiment. The results showed that the room size affected cue preference using conflicting cues but did not affect response accuracy using single cues at testing. These results support the retrieval hypothesis. The results were discussed in terms of the effects of cue salience and stability on cue interaction in reorientation.
Article
We employed a standard reference memory task to study the involvement of the hippocampal formation (HF) of domestic chicks that used the boundary geometry of a test environment to orient to and locate a reward. Using the immediate early gene product c-Fos as a neuronal activity marker, we found enhanced HF activation in chicks that learned to locate rewarded corners using the shape of a rectangular arena compared to chicks trained to solve the task by discriminating local features in a square-shaped arena. We also analysed neuronal activity in the medial part of the medial Striatum (mMSt). Surprisingly, in mMSt we observed a reverse pattern, with higher activity in the chicks that were trained to locate the goal by local features. Our results identify two seemingly parallel, memory systems in chicks, with HF central to the processing of spatial-geometrical information and mMSt important in supporting local feature discrimination. This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved. © 2015 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.
Article
Full-text available
Recent work has shown that in place-finding tasks rats rely on the geometric relations between the goal object and the shape of the environment. We tested young chickens (Gallus gallus domesticus) on similar tasks in a reference memory paradigm to determine whether differences exist between species in the ability to use geometric and nongeometric spatial information. The main findings were that chicks: (a) encoded and used both geometric and nongeometric (featural) information; (b) did not use the overall spatial arrangement of the features; (c) relied primarily on nongeometric cues when faced with contradictory information. Two mechanisms are evident in chicks’ spatial representations: a metric frame for encoding the spatial arrangement of surfaces as surfaces and a cue-guidance system for encoding conspicuous landmarks near the target. The possibility of hierarchical organization and species differences in these two mechanisms are discussed.
Article
Full-text available
Rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulata) were subjected to a place finding task in a rectangular room perfectly homogeneous and without distinctive featural information. Results of Experiment 1 show that monkeys rely on the large-scale geometry of the room to retrieve a food reward. Experiments 2 and 3 indicate that subjects use also nongeometric information (colored wall) to reorient. Data of Experiments 4 and 5 suggest that monkeys do not use small angular cues but that they are sensitive to the size of the cues (Experiments 6, 7, and 8). Our findings strengthen the idea that a mechanism based on the geometry of the environment is at work in several mammalian species. In addition, the present data offer new perspectives on spatial cognition in animals that are phylogenetically close to humans. Specifically, the joint use of both geometric and landmark-based cues by rhesus monkeys tends to demonstrate that spatial processing became more flexible with evolution.
Chapter
Full-text available
This chapter argues that human and animal minds indeed depend on a collection of domain-specific, task-specific, and encapsulated cognitive systems: on a set of cognitive 'modules' in Fodor's sense. It also argues that human and animal minds are endowed with domain-general, central systems that orchestrate the information delivered by core knowledge systems. The chapter begins by reviewing the literature on spatial reorientation in animals and in young children, arguing that spatial reorientation bears the hallmarks of core knowledge and of modularity. It then considers studies of older children and adults, arguing that human spatial representations change qualitatively over development and show capacities not found in any other species. Finally, it presents two new experiments that investigate the role of emerging spatial language in uniquely human navigation performance.
Article
Full-text available
Rats with entorhinal cortex lesions were trained in two versions of the place navigation task in the Morris water maze. In the distal condition, they had to locate the hidden platform on the basis of remote landmarks, while in the proximal condition, they had to rely only on a configuration of proximal objects, placed directly in the pool. Entorhinal rats were impaired in using distal landmarks but were able to use proximal landmarks to navigate toward the platform. These results suggest that the use of distal and proximal landmarks during navigation involves activation of different neural structures. They also suggest, in agreement with previous data, that there are two distinct landmark-processing systems, one devoted to the processing of proximal landmarks and the other to the processing of distal landmarks.
Chapter
The idea that the language we speak influences the way we think has evoked perennial fascination and intense controversy. According to the strong version of this hypothesis, called the Sapir-Whorf hypothesis after the American linguists who propounded it, languages vary in their semantic partitioning of the world, and the structure of one's language influences how one understands the world. Thus speakers of different languages perceive the world differently. Although the last two decades have been marked by extreme skepticism concerning the possible effects of language on thought, recent theoretical and methodological advances in cognitive science have given the question new life. Research in linguistics and linguistic anthropology has revealed striking differences in cross-linguistic semantic patterns, and cognitive psychology has developed subtle techniques for studying how people represent and remember experience. It is now possible to test predictions about how a given language influences the thinking of its speakers. Language in Mind includes contributions from both skeptics and believers and from a range of fields. It contains work in cognitive psychology, cognitive development, linguistics, anthropology, and animal cognition. The topics discussed include space, number, motion, gender, theory of mind, thematic roles, and the ontological distinction between objects and substances. ContributorsMelissa Bowerman, Eve Clark, Jill de Villiers, Peter de Villiers, Giyoo Hatano, Stan Kuczaj, Barbara Landau, Stephen Levinson, John Lucy, Barbara Malt, Dan Slobin, Steven Sloman, Elizabeth Spelke, and Michael Tomasello Bradford Books imprint
Chapter
In recent years there has been a shift within developmental psychology away from examining the cognitive systems at different ages, to trying to understand exactly what are the mechanisms that generate change. What kind of learning mechanisms and representational changes drive cognitive development? How can the imaging techniques available help us to understand these mechanisms? This new volume in the highy cited and critically acclaimed Attention and Performance series is the first to provide a systematic investigation into the processes of change in mental development. It brings together world class scientists to address brain and cognitive development at several different levels, including phylogeny, genetics, neurophysiology, brain imaging, behavior, and computational modeling, across both typically and atypically developing populations. Presenting original new research from the frontiers of cognitive neuroscience, this book will have a substantial impact in this field, as well as on developmental psychology and developmental neuroscience.
Article
Two experiments were conducted to investigate the development of children's cognitive maps of large-scale environments. Kindergarten (Mean Age = 5−7), second (Mean Age = 7−7), and fifth (Mean Age = 10−7) graders walked through a large model town and were then required to construct the layout of building in that model from memory. Accuracy of construction increased as a function both of developmental level and repeated encounters with the layout. In Experiment 1, the separate effects of repeatedly walking through the town and repeatedly constructing it both increased the accuracy of the children's reconstructions. In Experiment 2, walking through the environment was no more effective than merely viewing it repeatedly. Young children's accuracy in bounded space (Experiment 1) was far more accurate than their performance in unbounded space (Experiment 2), while older children's accuracy was relatively unaffected by this variable.