ArticlePDF Available

Failure to Replicate: Testing a Growth Mindset Intervention for College Student Success

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Interventions surrounding mindset have recently been applied as a tool for student success in higher education. The current study tested the efficacy of a growth mindset intervention at a university with a diverse student population. Using gateway math and introductory psychology courses, students were randomly assigned to receive a mindset message or one endorsing study skills. Dependent variables were course grade, term GPA, term credit hours earned, and retention to subsequent terms. Analyses using the full sample, minority sample, Pell-eligible, and first-generation college students did not yield meaningful differences in students’ academic success between the intervention and control groups. Further research should investigate why mindset intervention has proven successful with other populations not represented in the present study.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at
https://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=hbas20
Basic and Applied Social Psychology
ISSN: (Print) (Online) Journal homepage: https://www.tandfonline.com/loi/hbas20
Failure to Replicate: Testing a Growth Mindset
Intervention for College Student Success
Caitlin Brez , Eric M. Hampton , Linda Behrendt , Liz Brown & Josh Powers
To cite this article: Caitlin Brez , Eric M. Hampton , Linda Behrendt , Liz Brown & Josh Powers
(2020): Failure to Replicate: Testing a Growth Mindset Intervention for College Student Success,
Basic and Applied Social Psychology, DOI: 10.1080/01973533.2020.1806845
To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1080/01973533.2020.1806845
Published online: 12 Aug 2020.
Submit your article to this journal
View related articles
View Crossmark data
Failure to Replicate: Testing a Growth Mindset Intervention for College
Student Success
Caitlin Brez
a
, Eric M. Hampton
b
, Linda Behrendt
b
, Liz Brown
b
, and Josh Powers
c
a
University of North Carolina Asheville;
b
Indiana State University;
c
William Patterson University
ABSTRACT
Interventions surrounding mindset have recently been applied as a tool for student success
in higher education. The current study tested the efficacy of a growth mindset intervention
at a university with a diverse student population. Using gateway math and introductory
psychology courses, students were randomly assigned to receive a mindset message or one
endorsing study skills. Dependent variables were course grade, term GPA, term credit hours
earned, and retention to subsequent terms. Analyses using the full sample, minority sample,
Pell-eligible, and first-generation college students did not yield meaningful differences in
studentsacademic success between the intervention and control groups. Further research
should investigate why mindset intervention has proven successful with other populations
not represented in the present study.
As educators, one of our primary concerns is student
success. Higher education is increasing its focus on
this by creating offices and administrators who design
and deploy programs and activities to improve student
retention and graduation rates. At its core, these
changes are intended to help students navigate the
college experience, find resources and strategies to
help them in their courses, and support student learn-
ing. As part of this effort, institutions of higher educa-
tion are seeking cost-effective resources and
interventions that impact their studentssuccess.
Recently, one direction that these interventions have
taken has focused on the growth mindset theory
(Dweck, 2006).
In 2006, Carol Dweck published her popular book
Mindset, which outlines the theory and describes how
peoples beliefs about themselves can influence them
in a variety of domains such as business, relationships,
and academics. She describes two types of mindsets: a
growth mindset and a fixed mindset. With the growth
mindset (also called incremental theory), individuals
realize that their talents and abilities are subject to
change, specifically growth and improvement, as a
result of effort and learning. From this perspective, a
person is not born smart or athletic or artistic, but
rather through practice, hard work, and effort, can
develop these abilities. In contrast, the fixed mindset
(also called entity theory) is the belief that our
talents and abilities are inborn and static. While some
people have often conceptualized these as two catego-
ries of thinking, more recently researchers have con-
ceptualized these as opposite ends of a spectrum and
suggest that one can either possess aspects of both or
lean toward one pole or the other depending on the
situation (Dweck, 2015).
Beyond the value of understanding peoples motiv-
ation, this theory has stimulated research demonstrat-
ing that different mindsets can lead to different
outcomes and having a growth mindset can improve
learning outcomes. When children are praised for
their effort and the process of learning, children will
seek out more learning goals (i.e., they want more
challenging work as it will be an opportunity to
learn), have higher task performance, show greater
persistence on work, and will rate tasks as more
enjoyable (Mueller & Dweck, 1998). Additionally,
researchers have found similar results in middle
school math students (Blackwell et al., 2007). These
findings suggest that mindsets can influence other
internal belief states such as motivation and goals, but
can also influence behaviors as well.
Mindset intervention research
If mindsets can influence behavior, then it seems
logical that this is one potential source of intervention.
CONTACT Caitlin Brez cbrez@unca.edu Department of Psychology, University of North Carolina Asheville, 1 University Hts., Asheville, NC 28804-
3251, USA.
ß2020 Taylor & Francis Group, LLC
BASIC AND APPLIED SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY
https://doi.org/10.1080/01973533.2020.1806845
In fact, researchers have spent over 10 years investigat-
ing this question (Paunesku et al., 2015). In one of
the first studies of this kind, Dweck and colleagues
created an 8-week intervention in which they taught
middle school math students about the brain, its mal-
leability, and the potential to improve skills through
neural changes (Blackwell et al., 2007). The control
group did similar activities but never learned about
the growth mindset material. They found that the stu-
dents in the intervention group (i.e., growth mindset)
showed a positive change in motivation and their
math grades improved. As noted earlier, these positive
findings from the intervention have led researchers to
apply this intervention to diverse populations and out-
comes such as adolescents with mental health prob-
lems (Miu & Yeager, 2015; Schleider & Weisz, 2018),
adolescents working on personal development through
an outdoor adventure course (OBrien & Lomas,
2017) and parents trying to improve their childrens
reading and writing scores (Anderson & Nielson,
2016). However, the majority of these interventions
(also known as lay theory interventions,”“social psy-
chological interventions,or implicit theories of intel-
ligence interventions) have been targeted at students
in academic settings focused on promoting academic
outcomes such as grades (see Yeager & Walton, 2011
for a review of these studies).
While this work originated in K-12 education,
researchers have begun to appreciate the potential that
these interventions may have for higher education.
Growth mindset interventions have been effective at
improving studentsgrades and/or GPA (Aronson
et al., 2002; Yeager, Walton, et al., 2016), increasing
student retention (Yeager, Walton, et al., 2016), and
enjoyment and/or engagement in academics (Aronson
et al., 2002). Furthermore, these effects are most pro-
nounced for students from disadvantaged back-
grounds such as racial minorities and persons from
low-income backgrounds (Aronson et al., 2002; Good
et al., 2003; Rattan et al., 2015; Spitzer & Aronson,
2015; Yeager et al., 2014; Yeager, Romero, et al., 2016;
Yeager, Walton, et al., 2016). Another important find-
ing from this body of work is that these interventions
also are particularly relevant at periods of transitions
such as from high school to college (Yeager, Walton,
et al., 2016). However, this research has not been con-
ducted at an institution similar to the one for the pre-
sent study, which has comparatively larger numbers of
racial minorities, low-income students, and first-
generation students, as well as a lower level of admis-
sion selectivity. As some of the research suggests, this
is the population that may perhaps receive the greatest
benefit from these types of interventions (Aronson
et al., 2002; Good et al., 2003; Rattan et al., 2015;
Spitzer & Aronson, 2015; Yeager et al., 2014; Yeager,
Romero, et al., 2016; Yeager, Walton, et al., 2016), yet
this population remains understudied especially in
higher education.
Despite the positive evidence for these interven-
tions, research is beginning to surface which questions
the value of these interventions. Several studies have
failed to find positive effects for growth mindset inter-
ventions (Burnette et al., 2018; Dixson et al., 2017;
Schmidt et al., 2017) or the effects have been short-
lived (Orosz et al., 2017). Sisk et al. (2018) published
a meta-analysis highlighting the lack of effects. In
their first set of analyses, the authors looked at the
relationship between individualsmindsets and aca-
demic achievement. In this set of studies, there were
no interventions, but rather the intent was to study
the relationship between individualspreexisting
mindsets and academic outcomes. Of the 129 studies
that they analyzed, only 37% found a positive relation-
ship between mindset and academic outcomes.
Furthermore, 58% of the studies found no relationship
and 6% found a negative relationship between mindset
and academic outcomes (Sisk et al., 2018). The
authors noted that this relationship may be stronger
for children and adolescents, but does not seem to
hold as well for adults. In the second set of analyses,
the authors looked at the relationship between growth
mindset interventions and academic outcomes. They
found 29 studies of this type, and of these, only 12%
had a positive effect, meaning that the growth mindset
intervention improved academic achievement. In fact,
86% of the studies found no effect of the intervention
and 2% found a negative effect of the intervention
(Sisk et al., 2018). Consistent with earlier published
studies, the authors found that the interventions
seemed to work for low SES populations, but not
higher SES populations.
Focus of study
The study reported here tested a growth mindset
intervention at a Midwestern, regional university.
Many of the growth mindset interventions being con-
ducted at institutions of higher education were con-
ducted at either Ivy League schools or community
colleges. Our student population does not match the
demographics of the former with respect to our com-
paratively larger numbers of racial minorities, low-
income students, and first-generation students, as well
as our lower level of admission selectivity. For this
2 C. BREZ ET AL.
reason, we believe our students represent a population
that has not been addressed by growth mindset inter-
ventions and one that may benefit the most from this
type of intervention as supported by previous research
(Aronson et al., 2002; Good et al., 2003; Rattan et al.,
2015; Sisk et al., 2018; Spitzer & Aronson, 2015;
Yeager et al., 2014; Yeager, Romero, et al., 2016;
Yeager, Walton, et al., 2016). Through this random-
ized control trial, we tested the effectiveness of a
growth mindset intervention to improve students
course grades and GPA.
Materials and methods
Participants
The population for this project included students
enrolled in gateway math and psychology courses.
These courses were selected as they have particularly
high drop/fail/withdrawal (DFW) rates on this cam-
pus. As these are courses where students typically
struggle, the goal was to target these students for the
intervention to maximally impact their performance
in the course and their long-term success. These
courses were examined at three levels for math,
remedial (MATH 035; Fundamentals of Algebra II),
non-STEM (MATH 102; Quantitative Literacy), and
STEM and select other majors (MATH 115; College
Algebra), and within psychology, (PSY 101; General
Psychology). Such courses are routinely taught at col-
leges and universities throughout the country. At the
focal institution for this study, approximately 18% of
all students enrolled in MATH 035 received a D, an
F, or dropped the course annually. With regard to
MATH 102, 24% of all students enrolled received a D,
an F, or dropped the course annually. For MATH
115, 40% of enrolled students received a D, an F, or
dropped the course annually. Finally, 28% of enrolled
students received a D, an F, or dropped PSY
101 annually.
The sample for the study consisted of 2,135 under-
graduate students enrolled in the three levels of math
across three academic terms, Spring 2015, Fall 2015,
and Spring 2016 as well as 733 undergraduate stu-
dents enrolled in Psychology 101 across two academic
terms, Fall 2016 and Spring 2017. The total number
of students in these courses during this time period
was 4465, and of those, 2,607 (58.4%) participated in
this study. The students were majority freshmen and
demographically diverse in areas of critical student
mass on the campus, namely approximately 54% were
White, 42% were African American, and 48% were
low income (supported by a Pell grant).
Approximately 28% were the first generation, defined
as neither parent having a college degree.
Approximately, 50.2% of the sample were female
(based on the data for which gender was known).
Response rates to the online activity and survey
were not recorded. Students could leave the survey at
any time and take as little or much time to complete
the intervention/activity as desired. The data pre-
sented here represent participants who at least started
the online activity. However, students could only par-
ticipate in the online intervention/activity one time.
For example, if they took the online intervention in
their math class and then enrolled in psychology dur-
ing a later semester, they would receive a prompt
informing them that they had already participated.
Likewise, if they failed a class or retook a class, their
data would only be included in the analyses once. In
terms of the larger context of this intervention, we
continued to collect data on course grades, GPA, and
retention throughout the duration of the study period,
and no one was excluded once they were assigned to
either the treatment or control condition.
Measures
Dependent measures
Our dependent measures are typical outcome meas-
ures reported for these types of intervention studies
and included course grade, term GPA, and term credit
hours earned. These variables were measured on a
continuous scale. Letter grades for the course were
transformed into GPA units (A ¼4.0; A¼3.7; Bþ
¼3.3; B ¼3.0; B¼2.7; Cþ¼2.3; C ¼2.0; Dþ¼
1.7; D ¼1.0; D¼0.7; F ¼0.0) for analysis. The term
GPA was for the term of the intervention experiment.
Following the intervention, these measures were col-
lected at the end of the intervention term and once at
the end of each semester following.
Design and procedures
The study design was a randomized, controlled trial
such that students were randomly assigned at the
beginning to either the treatment or control condition.
All eligible students were randomly assigned to each
condition, and then once students were invited to par-
ticipate, they would be directed to the correct online
survey (either treatment or control). Prior to inviting
students, the section instructorswillingness to partici-
pate in the study was secured. Students were asked to
log into a website in which they were randomly
assigned to a treatment or control condition. Using an
BASIC AND APPLIED SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY 3
online survey platform, treatment condition students
were provided instructions asking them to read an
article about how the brain can grow stronger through
effort and how difficult subjects such as math (psych-
ology in the case of those students) can be mastered
as a result, and that anyone can learn math (or psych-
ology). The article was similar in content to previous
mindset interventions (e.g., Blackwell et al., 2007) and
was a little over one page in length. It provided scien-
tific evidence that the brain changes as a result of
learning. It also described how math (or psychology)
practice could lead to an improvement in skills and
why making mistakes help us learn more. They were
then invited to identify the main point of the article
from a list of three selection options. We did not
exclude any participants based on their responses to
this question because we provided the correct answer
to ensure appropriate interpretation. From there, they
were asked to write a letter to a future student
describing how they could learn math (or psychology)
and that these letters would be shared with future stu-
dents. This exercise was designed to help participants
reflect on the content of the article and hopefully
increase the retention of the main concepts. There
was no incentive for participants to complete this
exercise, but in both math and psychology courses,
the content was embedded within the course struc-
turefor math, it was conducted within the classroom
and for psychology, it was listed as an assignment for
students to complete.
The control condition students followed a similar
procedure, although in this case, they read a general art-
icle about math (or psychology), offering basic guidance
on seeking help such as through tutoring. There was no
mention of growth mindset or concepts related to this
theory in the control condition. The control condition
was designed to be as similar to the treatment condition
(e.g., reading length, questions asked, reflection activity)
without the mention of a growth mindset.
Additionally, a team of external evaluators moni-
tored the project for adherence to scientific best prac-
tices that met the What Works Clearinghouse
standards for research (U.S. Department of
Education, n.d.).
Results
As shown in Table 1, we calculated descriptive statis-
tics for our dependent measures (final grade for the
course, term GPA, and the number of earned hours
for the semester) for the entire sample as well as by
condition (treatment vs. control). As can be seen from
these data, there are little differences across conditions
for all three outcome variables. Standardized differen-
ces are presented as positive values when in agree-
ment with the theory (treatment greater than control)
and negative values when contrary to the theory (con-
trol greater than treatment). Standardized differences
across treatment conditions were very small for all
outcomes (final grade d¼0.01; term GPA d¼
0.02; earned hours d¼0.02). There is a minimal
effect of a growth mindset message on improving
these outcomes.
Minority student analyses
Given previous literature noting the particular benefits
for students from disadvantaged or marginalized back-
grounds, course grade (n¼846), term GPA (n¼892),
and term earned credit hours (n¼920) were com-
pared across treatment and control conditions for just
the sub-sample of minority students, defined as stu-
dents identifying as any ethnicity other than White.
These data are provided in Table 2. As in the full
sample, the differences between the treatment and
control conditions are slight and, if anything, favor
the control condition. Standardized differences across
treatment conditions were small to very small for all
outcomes (final grade d¼0.10; term GPA d¼
0.08; earned hours d¼0.01). There is little
Table 1. Descriptive statistics for dependent measures for the
full sample as well as for each condition.
Outcome nMean SD Skewness Kurtosis
Full sample 2,607
Final grade 2,639 2.39 1.39 0.54 0.97
Term GPA 2,821 2.71 0.88 0.85 0.24
Term earned hours 2,865 12.88 16.08 1.35 2.31
Treatment condition 1,314
Final grade 1,203 2.40 1.48 0.53 1.04
Term GPA 1,292 2.72 0.95 0.85 0.22
Term Earned hours 1,311 12.89 4.00 1.28 2.74
Control condition 1,293
Final grade 1,203 2.42 1.37 0.59 0.87
Term GPA 1,275 2.74 0.93 0.89 0.35
Term earned hours 1,293 12.96 3.95 1.42 2.63
Table 2. Descriptive statistics for dependent measures for
minority students.
Outcome nMean SD Skewness Kurtosis
Treatment condition 457
Final grade 417 2.02 1.48 0.16 1.39
Term GPA 454 2.50 0.90 0.56 0.15
Term earned hours 457 12.49 3.71 1.08 1.39
Control condition 501
Final grade 463 2.16 1.41 0.34 1.17
Term GPA 492 2.57 0.94 0.72 0.20
Term earned hours 500 12.51 4.04 1.33 1.82
4 C. BREZ ET AL.
evidence of any benefit of the growth mindset inter-
vention for minority students.
Pell-eligible student analyses
Course grade (n¼1,151), term GPA (n¼1,201), and
term earned credit hours (n¼1,249) were compared
across treatment and control conditions for Pell-eli-
gible students, the second category of disadvantaged
or marginalized student. These data are presented in
Table 3. Standardized differences across treatment
conditions were very small or non-existent for all out-
comes (final grade d¼0; term GPA d¼þ0.03;
earned hours d¼þ0.01). There are little or no differ-
ences between the treatment and control groups on
these outcome variables for Pell-eligible students.
First-generation college student analyses
The final analysis involved a third student category,
first-generation students, who the literature also notes
as often historically marginalized or disadvantaged in
higher education. These data are presented in Table 4.
Standardized differences across treatment conditions
were small for all outcomes (final grade d¼þ0.11;
term GPA d¼þ0.12; earned hours d¼þ0.07). As
with previous analyses, there is little difference for
these outcome variables between the treatment and
control conditions, in this case for first-gener-
ation students.
Discussion
This study tested the effectiveness of a growth mindset
intervention on studentsacademic success (measured
by course grades, term GPA, and earned credit hours)
for students at a Midwestern, regional university. The
pattern of findings is clear that the intervention had lit-
tle impact on studentsacademic success even among
sub-samples of students who are traditionally assumed
to benefit from this type of intervention (e.g., minority,
low income, and first-generation students).
These findings support some of the emerging litera-
ture that demonstrates that growth mindset interven-
tions may not be as effective as once thought
(Burnette et al., 2018; Dixson et al., 2017; Sisk et al.,
2018; Schmidt et al., 2017). While the growth mindset
has been used in K-12 schools for many years, its
application in the realm of higher education is more
recent. The proposition that a one-time intervention
at the postsecondary level will result in long-term
measurable student outcomes was not supported in
the present study. Despite the student body make-up
thought to be most likely to benefit from an interven-
tion (e.g., first-time college, low income, racially
minority), the outcomes of the interventions were not
evident across these populations.
The findings from the present study provide add-
itional evidence to support questioning the effect of
growth mindset in student success and retention
efforts in higher education, at least in a one-time
intervention form that some literature suggests is suf-
ficient. The research design followed protocols used in
earlier mindset intervention research; however, the
student population involved in the present study had
not been addressed in the previous studies.
Additionally, we included first-time college students, a
time of transition when interventions are purported to
be especially applicable (Yeager, Walton, et al., 2016).
Despite previous findings showing the effectiveness of
a one-time intervention for this transition time
(Yeager & Walton, 2011), our data did not support
this conclusion.
Perhaps the lack of support for this intervention is
due to a limitation in our sample. Approximately 41%
of the students invited to participate in the interven-
tion chose not to or were prevented from participat-
ing a second time. While the sample that did
participate generally matched the demographics of the
entire study body, it is possible that the group that
chose not to participate differed significantly in a
meaningful way that could be masking or eliminating
any possible effect of the intervention. Despite this,
we used a randomized controlled trial to minimize
Table 3. Descriptive statistics for dependent measures for Pell
eligible students.
Outcome nMean SD Skewness Kurtosis
Treatment condition 650
Final grade 593 2.23 1.44 0.39 1.22
Term GPA 634 2.59 0.93 0.66 0.11
Term earned hours 648 12.71 4.04 1.40 1.86
Control condition 643
Final grade 592 2.23 1.36 0.41 1.03
Term GPA 633 2.56 0.97 0.80 0.05
Term earned hours 642 12.68 4.14 1.50 1.86
Table 4. Descriptive statistics for dependent measures for
first-generation students.
Outcome nMean SD Skewness Kurtosis
Treatment condition 345
Final grade 315 2.45 1.46 0.57 1.06
Term GPA 338 2.75 0.96 0.82 0.03
Term earned hours 344 12.98 3.86 1.51 2.37
Control condition 349
Final grade 317 2.30 1.31 0.52 0.85
Term GPA 344 2.64 0.91 0.85 0.32
Term earned hours 349 12.71 3.91 1.34 2.35
BASIC AND APPLIED SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY 5
bias across the two groups. Our study was evaluated
by a team of external evaluators who relied on the
What Works Clearinghouse (WWC) standards for
research, which maintains the highest standards for
education intervention research (U.S. Department of
Education, n.d.).
Additionally, the intervention was conducted in
two different disciplinesmath, in which placement is
made by demonstrated ability; and psychology, which
is open to students of all abilities without prerequisite
demonstration of competence. The first three inter-
ventions were completed with students enrolled in a
math class, a discipline that previous studies had
examined. Not only was math chosen for this study
because it was a domain that has been addressed pre-
viously through mindset interventions (Blackwell
et al., 2007), but math is a field where fixed mindsets
seem to be rampant (e.g., Im just not a math per-
son). Thus, it seems like this would be a field where
there is great potential to change mindsets. Yet, these
cohorts evidenced little difference between the treat-
ment and control groups. One concern was that math
is also a field in which students have had years of
experience in which this growth mindset (or fixed
mindset) can build. Most students have been taking
math since they first started schooling. It might be a
real challenge to alter over 12 years of experience in
this field, in which studentsmindsets have been built
and reinforced over the years. Therefore, the research-
ers decided to try the intervention in a different
discipline without this same educational history
psychology. However, once again little effect of the
intervention was found in psychology. The difference
in discipline did not generate meaningful differences
in any of the variables (e.g., Pell, minority, first-time
college). Thus, it appears that even in a discipline
where the opportunity to cement a fixed mindset is
less or where the potential to introduce a growth
mindset is greater, the intervention was still not effect-
ive in changing the student success outcomes that
were measured.
Spitzer and Aronson (2015) asserted that factors
beyond belief about how intelligence affects academic
success are also important to student success, and
may provide the untested explanation for no differen-
ces between groups. Some examples include exogen-
ous factors that are not captured by a simple measure
of race/ethnicity, first-generation status, or a cut cat-
egory on income (i.e., Pell or not). Furthermore, the
proximity of the campus to home may also be a rele-
vant unmeasured factor as might the ease of transfer-
ability, a consideration relevant to the term retention
finding. There is also a rich literature on the topic of
toxic stress (Shern et al., 2016), a phenomenon these
researchers know is common at the focal institution,
and might also be difficult or impossible to overcome
from a simple psychological intervention. Related to
this point, some scholars report the importance of sat-
isfying individualsbasic needs (e.g., Maslows hier-
archy of needs) prior to focusing on academic
motivations and goals (Rich, 2011 and Burdenski &
Faulkner, 2010). Particularly for students of low socio-
economic status (SES), priorities often focus on deal-
ing with nonacademic needs and stress first. Thus, an
intervention focused on growth mindset might not be
motivating enough or might not focus on the funda-
mental needs at the moment for these students. Given
that our student population experiences financial
stress, toxic stress, food insecurity, and other concerns
related to their low SES, this may be a possible
explanation for the lack of findings in the cur-
rent study.
Alternatively, it is possible, as suggested by our
data and othersdata (Sisk et al., 2018) that these
types of growth mindset interventions are not as
effective as originally claimed. Perhaps the message or
intervention is too simple; student success is a compli-
cated topic and many factors, motivations, and behav-
iors may all play a role in these outcome variables
(Karlen et al., 2019). As noted above, many factors
can influence studentsperceptions, experience with
the transition to college, and their ability to success-
fully navigate this environment (i.e., fear of failure;
Bartels & Ryan, 2013). It is possible that a one-time
intervention cannot overcome over 18 years of educa-
tion and experience that may fuel some of the percep-
tions and motivations that this intervention is trying
to overcome. Perhaps these interventions may be bet-
ter for younger ages, although research is not support-
ing that claim (Li & Bates, 2019; Sisk et al., 2018).
Another possibility is that these interventions are not
changing peoples mindsets. While we are measuring
effects on large-scale academic outcomes, the underly-
ing assumption is that these interventions are affecting
individualsmindsets (i.e., encouraging more growth
mindset thinking; Bahnik & Vranka, 2017). Related to
this point, it is possible that a large number or even
the majority of individuals start high on the growth
mindset, thus limiting the effectiveness of a short-
term intervention. Two ways to address this issue
would be to better understand our current measure-
ments of mindset and to develop alternate measure-
ments of this construct. Furthermore, perhaps
methodological differences that may be difficult to
6 C. BREZ ET AL.
capture in writing may exist and account for some of
the differences between the current study and previ-
ous studies that have found a positive effect on
growth mindset interventions. Differences in data ana-
lysis procedures could reflect differences in the report-
ing of the outcomes. In the present study, we relied
heavily on our randomized controlled design to allow
us to make simple comparisons across the two condi-
tions. In our analysis comparing the two conditions,
there was a negligible effect of the intervention on
student outcomes.
If these growth mindset interventions are not as
effective as claimed, then this necessitates a reevalua-
tion of Dwecks growth mindset theory. Several possi-
bilities exist here. At one extreme is the possibility
that people do not have these different types of mind-
sets (fixed or growth). These may be manifestations of
other aspects of our psychological experience such as
other motivations, like approach or avoidance (Bartels
& Ryan, 2013). Research exists that demonstrates how
our motivations can be shaped by fear of failure, self-
efficacy, grit, and locus of control and how these
motivations can affect academic performance
(Burdenski & Faulkner, 2010; Buzzetto-Hollywood
et al., 2019; Ciani et al., 2011; De Castella et al., 2013;
Kannangara et al., 2018; Karlen et al., 2019). If we do
not have these mindsets as proposed, these alternative
psychological theories could explain some of the
behavioral and psychological effects that are attributed
to mindsets. Alternatively, if we do not have these
mindsets, then this could explain why research is fail-
ing to support the value of these interventions.
Finally, these alternative psychological theories could
better explain how our motivations could affect aca-
demic outcomes.
Another, and more plausible, possibility to reevalu-
ate mindset theory is that the presence of differing
mindsets (fixed and growth) are valid, but that the
ability to use, manipulate or alter these mindsets to
the benefit of others is not possible. To use a fixed
mindset argument, these mindsets may be more like
personality traits that are not as malleable as Dweck
suggests. Mindsets might be harder to change and
alter than previously thought (Li & Bates, 2019). One
of the attractions of the mindset interventions is that
they are relatively easyquick, inexpensive, easily
administered. Many educators were likely attracted to
the feasibility of implementing these interventions at
such a low investment. However, this might be a situ-
ation in which the intervention was too good to be
true.If mindsets are not as malleable as suggested,
then this simple, one-time intervention might not be
enough dosage or enough power to alter or manipu-
late mindsets. For our population, again, these stu-
dents had approximately 18 years of lived experience
that likely contributed to and shaped their mindsets
regarding their academic ability. A brief intervention
might not be enough to overcome that experience.
The implications of this research are several. First,
research should continue in this field to better under-
stand the mindset theory and these types of interven-
tions. Our data, along with others, suggest that
mindset interventions are not effective in improving
academic outcomes, at least course grade, term GPA,
and earned credit hours. A second implication is that
schools and universities may want to consider with-
drawing any interventions of this kind or stop pursu-
ing any future interventions until we know more.
These interventions do not appear to be doing harm,
but any investment of time, energy, and money may
not be worthwhile. A third implication of these data
is that mindset theory may need reevaluation. More
thoughtful review and evaluation of the theory can
help us to better understand its merits and weaknesses
and its relevance to empirically support the psycho-
logical theory. We certainly hope that research will
continue in this field to help better understand both
the theory and its practical implications for
human behavior.
In conclusion, the lack of differences across all of
the measures calls into question the utility of brief,
one-time interventions focused on growth mindset, at
least for the kinds of students found at a regional uni-
versity. It is possible that these types of interventions
may be more effective for other types of student pop-
ulations and institutional settings, but further research
is needed. Future studies should also explore why the
intervention has been shown to be effective with K-12
students but produces mixed results with college stu-
dents. It is possible that these types of interventions,
when combined with other psychological messaging,
such as belongingness (Walton & Cohen, 2011;
Walton et al., 2015) or self-control (Duckworth &
Seligman, 2005; Tangney et al., 2004), or grit
(Duckworth, 2016), maybe more effective. Another
possible direction for future research is to identify dif-
ferent measures of student success, such as achieve-
ment scores or studentssubjective experience rather
than grades and GPA. Finally, it may be that multiple
psychological intervention dosesmight be needed to
realize an effect. Nevertheless, the present study calls
into question the universal use of growth mindset
interventions across sectors of higher education, des-
pite its practical and cost-effective appeal. And, given
BASIC AND APPLIED SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY 7
the importance of the regional state university for
educating the largest numbers of historically under-
served students in the 4-year sector in the United
States, it is important that research continues to
attempt to identify ways to help them remain in
school and graduate.
Acknowledgment
We appreciate Linda Fergusons generous support with
gathering institutional data for this project.
Funding
This study was supported by a First in the World Grant
through the Department of Education.
ORCID
Caitlin Brez http://orcid.org/0000-0002-7285-2961
References
Anderson, S. C., Nielson, H. S. (2016). Reading intervention
with a growth mindset approach improves childrens
skills. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences,
113, 1211112113.
Aronson, J., Fried, C. B., & Good, C. (2002). Reducing the
effects of stereotype threat on African American college
students by shaping theories of intelligence. Journal of
Experimental Social Psychology,38(2), 113125. https://
doi.org/10.1006/jesp.2001.1491
Bahnik, S., & Vranka, M. A. (2017). Growth mindset is not
associated with scholastic aptitude in a large sample of
university applicants. Personality and Individual
Differences,117, 139143.
Bartels, J. M., & Ryan, J. J. (2013). Fear of failure and
achievement goals: A canonical analysis. Journal of
Instructional Psychology,40,4249.
Blackwell, L. S., Trzesniewski, K. H., & Dweck, C. S. (2007).
Implicit theories of intelligence predict achievement
across an adolescent transition: A longitudinal study and
an intervention. Child Development,78(1), 246263.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.00995.x
Burdenski, T. K., & Faulkner, B. (2010). Empowering col-
lege students to satisfy their basic needs: Implications for
primary, secondary, and post-secondary educators.
International Journal of Choice Theory and Reality
Therapy,30,7397.
Burnette, J. L., Russell, M. V., Hoyt, C. L., Orvidas, K., &
Widman, L. (2018). An online growth mindset interven-
tion in a sample of rural adolescent girls. British Journal
of Educational Psychology,88(3), 428445. https://doi.org/
10.1111/bjep.12192
Buzzetto-Hollywood, N., Mitchell, B. C., & Hill, A. J.
(2019). Introducing a mindset intervention to improve
student success. Interdisciplinary Journal of E-Skills and
Lifelong Learning,15, 135155.
Ciani, K. D., Sheldon, K. M., Hilpert, J. C., & Easter, M. A.
(2011). Antecedents and trajectories of achievement goals:
A self-determination theory perspective. The British
Journal of Educational Psychology,81(2), 223243. https://
doi.org/10.1348/000709910X517399
De Castella, K., Byrne, D., & Covington, M. (2013).
Unmotivation or motivated to fail? A cross-cultural study
of achievement motivation, fear of failure, and student
disengagement. Journal of Educational Psychology,105(3),
861880. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0032464
Dixson, D. D., Roberson, C. C. B., & Worrell, F. C. (2017).
Psychosocial keys to African American achievement?
Examining the relationship between achievement and
psychosocial variables in high achieving African
Americans. Journal of Advanced Academics,28(2),
120140. https://doi.org/10.1177/1932202X17701734
Duckworth, A. (2016). Grit: The power of passion and perse-
verance. Scribner.
Duckworth, A. L., & Seligman, M. E. P. (2005). Self-discip-
line outdoes IQ in predicting academic performance of
adolescents. Psychological Science,16(12), 939944.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01641.x
Dweck, C. S. (2006). Mindset: The new psychology of success.
Ballantine Books.
Dweck, C. S. (2015). Carol Dweck revisits the growth
mindset,. Education Week,35,2024.
Good, C., Aronson, J., & Inzlicht, M. (2003). Improving
adolescentsstandardized test performance: An interven-
tion to reduce the effects of stereotype threat. Journal of
Applied Developmental Psychology,24(6), 645662.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appdev.2003.09.002
Kannangara, C. S., Allen, R. E., Waugh, G., Nahar, N.,
Zahraa, S., Khan, N., Rogerson, S., & Carson, J. (2018).
All that glitters is not grit: Three studies of grit in univer-
sity students. Frontiers in Psychology,9, 1539. https://doi.
org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01539
Karlen, Y., Suter, F., Hirt, C., & Merki, K. M. (2019). The
role of implicit theories in studentsgrit, achievement
goals, intrinsic and extrinsic motivation, and achievement
in the context of a long-term challenging task. Learning
and Individual Differences,74, 101757. https://doi.org/10.
1016/j.lindif.2019.101757
Li, Y., & Bates, T. C. (2019). You cant change your basic
ability, but you work at things, and thats how we get
hard things done: Testing the role of growth mindset on
response to setbacks, educational attainment, and cogni-
tive ability. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General,
148(9), 16401655. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000669
Miu, A. S., & Yeager, D. S. (2015). Preventing symptoms of
depression by teaching adolescents that people can
change: Effects of a brief incremental theory of personal-
ity intervention at 9-month follow-up. Clinical
Psychological Science,3(5), 726743. https://doi.org/10.
1177/2167702614548317
Mueller, C. M., & Dweck, C. S. (1998). Praise for intelli-
gence can undermine childrens motivation and perform-
ance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,75(1),
3352. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.75.1.33
OBrien, K., & Lomas, T. (2017). Developing a growth
mindset through outdoor personal development: Can an
intervention underpinned by psychology increase the
impact of an outdoor learning course for young people?
8 C. BREZ ET AL.
Journal of Adventure Education and Outdoor Learning,
17, 133147.
Orosz, G., P
eter-Szarka, S., B}
othe, B., T
oth-Kir
aly, I., &
Berger, R. (2017). How not to do a mindset intervention:
Learning from a mindset intervention among students
with good grades. Frontiers in Psychology,8, 311. https://
doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00311
Paunesku, D., Walton, G. M., Romero, C., Smith, E. N.,
Yeager, D. S., & Dweck, C. S. (2015). Mind-set interven-
tions are a scalable treatment for academic underachieve-
ment. Psychological Science,26(6), 784793. https://doi.
org/10.1177/0956797615571017
Rattan, A., Savani, K., Chugh, D., & Dweck, C. S. (2015).
Leveraging mindsets to promote academic achievement:
Policy recommendations. Perspect Psychol Sci,10(6),
721726. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691615599383
Rich, J. D. (2011). The use of pecuniary incentives for aca-
demic performance. Journal of Applied Global Research,
4, 3543.
Schleider, J., & Weisz, J. (2018). A single-session growth
mindset intervention for adolescent anxiety and depres-
sion: 9-month outcomes of a randomized trial. Journal of
Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied disciplines,
59(2), 160170. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12811
Schmidt, J. A., Shumow, L., & Kackar-Cam, H. Z. (2017).
Does mindset intervention predict studentsdaily experi-
ence in classrooms? A comparison of seventh and ninth
graderstrajectories. Journal of Youth and Adolescence,
46(3), 582602. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-016-0489-z
Shern, D. L., Blanch, A. K., & Steverman, S. M. (2016).
Toxic stress, behavioral health, and the next major era in
public health. The American Journal of Orthopsychiatry,
86(2), 109123. https://doi.org/10.1037/ort0000120
Sisk, V. F., Burgoyne, A. P., Sun, J., Butler, J. I., &
Macnamara, B. N. (2018). To what extent and under
which circumstances are growth mind-sets important to
academic achievement? Two meta-analyses. Psychological
Science,29(4), 549571. https://doi.org/10.1177/09567
97617739704
Spitzer, B., & Aronson, J. (2015). Minding and mending the
gap: Social psychological interventions to reduce educa-
tional disparities. The British Journal of Educational
Psychology,85(1), 118. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjep.
12067
Tangney, J. P., Baumeister, R. F., & Boone, A. L. (2004).
High self-control predicts good adjustment, less path-
ology, better grades, and interpersonal success. Journal of
Personality,72(2), 271324. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.
0022-3506.2004.00263.x
U.S. Department of Education. (n.d.) What works clearing-
house. https://ies.ed.gov/ncee/wwc/
Walton, G. M., & Cohen, G. L. (2011). A brief social-
belonging intervention improves academic and health
outcomes of minority students. Science,331(6023),
14471451. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1198364
Walton, G. M., Logel, C., Peach, J. M., Spencer, S. J., &
Zanna, M. P. (2015). Two brief interventions to mitigate
achilly climatetransform womens experience, relation-
ships, and achievement in engineering. Journal of
Educational Psychology,107(2), 468485. https://doi.org/
10.1037/a0037461
Yeager, D. S., Johnson, R., Spitzer, B. J., Trzesniewski,
K. H., Powers, J., & Dweck, C. S. (2014). The far-reach-
ing effects of believing people can change: Implicit theo-
ries of personality shape stress, health, and achievement
during adolescence. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology,106(6), 867884. https://doi.org/10.1037/
a0036335
Yeager, D. S., Romero, C., Paunesku, D., Hulleman, C. S.,
Schneider, B., Hinojosa, C., Lee, H. Y., OBrien, J., Flint,
K., Roberts, A., Trott, J., Greene, D., Walton, G. M., &
Dweck, C. S. (2016). Using design thinking to improve
psychological interventions: The case of the growth
mindset during the transition to high school. Journal of
Educational Psychology,108(3), 374391. https://doi.org/
10.1037/edu0000098
Yeager, D. S., & Walton, G. M. (2011). Social-psychological
interventions in education: Theyre not magic. Review of
Educational Research,81(2), 267301. https://doi.org/10.
3102/0034654311405999
Yeager, D. S., Walton, G. M., Brady, S. T., Akcinar, E. N.,
Paunesku, D., Keane, L., Kamentz, D., Ritter, G.,
Duckworth, A. L., Urstein, R., Gomez, E. M., Markus,
H. R., Cohen, G. L., & Dweck, C. S. (2016). Teaching a
lay theory before college narrows achievement gaps at
scale. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences,
113(24), E3341E3348. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.
1524360113
BASIC AND APPLIED SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY 9
... Researchers also found that some of the growth mindset claims failed to replicate in follow-up studies (e.g. see Brez et al, 2020), and they found some of the claims being made are coming from people that may be biased and/or have financial incentives to report positive outcomes (Macnamara & Burgoyne, 2022). ...
Preprint
Full-text available
A mindset type refers to the classification of a specific type of mindset. This systematic review and meta-analysis investigates all of the different mindset types that have been named throughout history in the title of scholarly publications. It was found that 1,882 different mindset types have been named, with a small number of those types becoming quite dominant in the field. It was also found mindset types seldom acknowledge the existence of one another, and they also seldom study how all mindset types are interrelated. It is recommended everyone in the field of mindset works together to break down scholarly silos, and widens their circle of acknowledgement, so they can embrace a diverse and comprehensive view of mindset theory that is inclusive of all mindset types.
... Once students receive strong motivation from their lecturer's intervention, they are likely to have a clearer purpose that will improve their overall outcome. Furthermore, it is essential to note that building a relationship between teacher and students can optimize the intervention (Balan & Sjöwall, 2023;Brez et al., 2020;Yeager & Dweck, 2020). ...
Article
Full-text available
This research discusses implementing general intelligence theory in the Visual Arts Education Department to improve students' creativity from social and cognitive development. The two most relevant approaches in visual arts education are general intelligence and multiple intelligence. This research selects general intelligence theory as the primary approach rather than multiple intelligence theory. Therefore, social development and cognitive development are discussed. This research method is a type of literature review, and the topic of study includes social development, cognitive development, growth mindset, rewards, intervention, and feedback. Moreover, the sources came from book chapters and articles from Elsevier, Sage, Routledge, etc. As a result, the findings of this research show (1) there are five general intelligence and nine multiple intelligence approaches; (2) Social development shows two approaches: active learning which consists of analyzing, defining, creating, and evaluating, while critical thinking consists of reading, writing, interpreting, and testing; (3) Cognitive development shows four approaches; growth mindset that consist of change perception and hardworking, rewards consist of extrinsic and intrinsic motivation, the intervention consists of consultation and motivation, and finally feedback consists of direct feedback and immediate feedback. Using a psychological approach to general intelligence, this teaching method in Visual Arts Education will improve students' creativity. Kecerdasan umum versus kecerdasan ganda: Perkembangan sosial dan kognitif dalam pendidikan seni rupa Abstrak Penelitian ini mendiskusikan implementasi dari jeneral intelligen dalam Program Studi Pendidikan Seni Rupa, yang bertujuan untuk mengembangkan kreativitas mahasiswa dari perkembangan sosial dan kognitif. Dalam pendidikan seni rupa, terdapat dua pendekatan yang paling relevan yaitu kecerdasan umum dan kecerdasan ganda. Dalam penelitian ini, teori kecerdasan umum dipilih sebagai pendekatan utama daripada teori kecerdasan ganda. Oleh karena itu, perkembangan sosial dan perkembangan kognitif didiskusikan. Metode penelitian ini adalah jenis tinjauan literatur, topiknya terdiri dari perkembangan sosial, perkembangan kognitif, pikiran berkembang, penghargaan, intervensi, dan umpan balik. Kemudian, sumbernya berasal dari buku chapter dan artikel dari journal bereputasi internasional seperti Elsevier, Sage, Routledge dan seterusnya.. Sehingga, temuan penelitian ini menunjukkan; (1) terdapat lima pendekatan kecerdasan umum dan sembilan pendekatan kecerdasan ganda; (2) Dalam perkembangan sosial terdapat dua pendekatan yaitu pembelajaran aktif yang terdiri dari menganalisis, menentukan, menciptakan, dan mengevaluasi, sementara pemikiran kritis terdiri dari membaca, menulis, menafsirkan, dan menguji;(3) Perkembangan kognitif menunjukkan empat pendekatan yaitu pola pikir berkembang yang terdiri dari mengubah persepsi dan kerja keras, imbalan yang terdiri dari motivasi ekstrinsik dan intrinsik, intervensi yang terdiri dari konsultasi dan motivasi, dan akhirnya umpan balik yang terdiri dari umpan balik langsung dan umpan balik segera. Metode pengajaran dalam Pendidikan Seni Rupa dari pendekatan psikologis kecerdasan umum akan meningkatkan kreativitas siswa.
... While adopting a growth mindset seems to be an impacting factor for achievement outcomes, mindset theory findings do not systematically replicate or result in contradictory findings, including on post-failure performance and academic persistence (Li & Bates, 2019;Macnamara & Rupani, 2017;Shedlosky-Shoemaker & Fautch, 2015) and academic achievement (Bahník & Vranka, 2017;Corradi et al., 2019). Another discrepancy across studies concerns intervention outcomes, which is subject to a fierce debate (Brez et al., 2020;Li & Bates, 2019;Macnamara, 2018;McCabe & Fleeson, 2016;Moreau, 2022;Yeager & Dweck, 2020;Yeager et al., 2019Yeager et al., , 2022. To shed some (systematic) light on these discrepancies, two recent meta-analyses were conducted: one by Sisk et al. (2018) and the other one by Costa and Faria (2018). ...
Article
Full-text available
In school settings, students’ mindset about intelligence (i.e., fixed versus growth mindset) and their sense of belonging to school (SBS) have both been shown to predict academic attainment. However, these constructs have rarely been examined together although both were found to be impacted by students’ socioeconomic status (SES). Across the literature, findings are inconsistent concerning this moderating effect of SES. In the present preregistered study, we used data from the French sample of the Programme for International Student Assessment 2018 (PISA; N = 6308) to examine whether growth mindset positively predicted SBS and whether this association was moderated by students’ SES. Results showed that growth mindset was positively associated with SBS. On the confirmatory linear regression analyses, we found no moderation effect of any of the SES indicators on the association between growth mindset and SBS. However, pre-registered supplementary multigroup analyses showed descriptively that this association was stronger for high than for low SES students and notably when SES indicators concerned family financial resources. Limitations of this research and perspectives for future studies are discussed, with a focus on why the literature should care about the different meanings and consequences of SES indicators.
... Mindset interventions have demonstrated an inconsistent degree of efficacy [21][22][23]. Yeager and Walton [12] claim that mindset interventions are remarkable tools for boosting academic achievement measures like GPA and test scores, and ref. [24] found that a short mindset intervention raised grades in lower-achieving students and increased enrollment in advanced math courses when the intervention message was congruent with peer norms. On the other hand, some recent research suggests that the claims pertaining to mindset's influence over measures of cognitive ability are too bold and that the investment in mindset interventions should be tempered [25]. ...
Article
Full-text available
There has been extensive research conducted on mindset and grit, involving both experimental and observational methods. However, the findings in the literature remain mixed. This should give educators and researchers pause from an intervention perspective—if we still do not have a good understanding of how mindset works, then more research is needed. We implemented a mindset intervention with undergraduate women to improve cognitive performance measures relevant to academic performance—working memory capacity and standardized test performance in math. To better understand how mindset interventions work, we also examined self-report measures (e.g., pertaining to academic attitudes and belonging) as well as post-intervention behavior. We expected the growth mindset intervention to significantly improve cognitive performance and to cause more positive academic attitudes and attitudinal change. The mindset intervention did change students’ beliefs about ability and also caused students to report higher grit overall (no condition difference), and to feel less belonging in terms of connection to their university—which was not in line with our hypotheses. We also found that the growth mindset intervention had no significant effects on improving WMC or standardized test performance. We discuss the implications of these findings and make suggestions for future work in this area.
... These interventions are especially important for students at critical developmental turning points, such as adolescence, to make sense of the challenges . However, Brez et al. (2020) warn against one-time interventions and suggest that continued interventions that combine a growth mindset with other psychological elements can be more effective in increasing achievement. Regarding immigrant students' science achievement, resilience was a predictor in the US, and attitudes toward school was a predictor in Australia; therefore, intervention studies might consider including these psychological elements alongside mindset in these countries. ...
Article
Full-text available
Plain Language Summary Immigrant Students’ Growth Mindset and Resilience In recent years, there has been a lot of interest in self-theories, such as growth mindset and resilience. These theories have a big impact on achievement and school-related motivation. This study looked at the relationship between immigrant students’ growth mindset, resilience, and science achievement in PISA 2018. The researchers also looked at whether attitudes toward school mediated this relationship. The researchers used secondary data from PISA 2018 for Australia, the UK, and the USA. They used structural equation modeling to analyze the relationship between self-theories and science achievement. The results showed that growth mindset had the strongest effect on science achievement for both immigrants and non-immigrants in all three countries. Resilience was positively related to science achievement for immigrants in the US, and attitudes toward school were positively related to science achievement for immigrants in Australia. The researchers could not confirm that attitudes toward school mediated the relationship between growth mindset, resilience, and science achievement. They speculate that self-theories might affect immigrant groups differently in different countries.
... Likewise, more consideration to heterogeneity also helps combat identity-based barriers because it puts the focus on differences in what students experience and how this impacts their performance. Currently, in the educational intervention literature, discussions on some of the most prominent topics (i.e., growth mindset and mindfulness interventions, and other culturally sensitive and inclusive practices) revolve around intervention efficacy or inefficacy (Bluth et al., 2016;Brez et al., 2020;Dunning et al., 2019;Yeager et al., 2019; also see Simons et al., 2016 on cognitive enhancementbased interventions, efficacy and research standards). Central to the debate is whether these interventions are worth continuing to implement in schools and in some cases, questions regarding whether it is useful to continue to conduct basic scientific research on these topics at all (see Burgoyne et al., 2020;Moreau et al., 2018;Sisk et al., 2018). ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Students racialized as Black often experience forms of marginalization and encounter unique obstacles in their educational paths. As a result, more inclusive and tailored forms of support should be developed. Working to better support Black students should be initiated through two complementary sides--through that of the instructor and the scientist. First, we consider the importance of identity-safety and detail identity-based barriers Black students experience in their learning environments. We then define the notion of cognitive universals and discuss how it limits the effectiveness of science and its translation to more inclusive practices. We argue that to strengthen tailored forms of student support, we must move beyond focusing only on cognitive universals and more carefully consider heterogeneity of effects. To support our argument, we present evidence-based strategies from cognitive and learning science and educational interventions research to combat identity-based barriers and boost learning gains. Overall, we urge instructors and scientists to consider a variety of techniques to create more identity-safe and inclusive learning environments for Black students.
... These competencies help university students face challenges more easily, thus promoting entrepreneurship and reducing dropout rates. [85], [97]- [99]. ...
Article
Students racialized as Black often experience forms of marginalization and encounter unique obstacles in their educational paths. As a result, more inclusive and tailored forms of support should be developed. Working to better support Black students should be initiated through two complementary sides—through that of the instructor and the scientist. First, we consider the importance of identity‐safety and detail identity‐based barriers Black students experience in their learning environments. We then define the notion of cognitive universals and discuss how it limits the effectiveness of science and its translation to more inclusive practices. We argue that to strengthen tailored forms of student support, we must move beyond focusing only on cognitive universals and more carefully consider heterogeneity of effects. To support our argument, we present evidence‐based strategies from cognitive and learning science and educational interventions research to combat identity‐based barriers and boost learning gains. Overall, we urge instructors and scientists to consider a variety of techniques to create more identity‐safe and inclusive learning environments for Black students.
Article
Full-text available
COVID‐19 poses a considerable threat to adolescent mental health. We investigated depression rates in teens from pre to post‐COVID. We also explored if leveraging a growth mindset intervention (“Healthy Minds”) could improve adolescent mental health outcomes during the pandemic, especially for adolescents experiencing the most distress. In Study 1, we recruited youth from schools in a rural southern community ( N = 239) and used a pre‐post design. In Study 2, we recruited an online sample ( N = 833) and used a longitudinal randomized control trial design to test the effectiveness of Healthy Minds. Across both studies, there is evidence of higher rates of depression in youth during COVID‐19, relative to pre‐pandemic numbers. In Study 1, the intervention effectively changed psychological and behavioral processes related to mental health, especially for adolescents experiencing greater COVID‐19 stress. However, in Study 2, the intervention failed to impact depression rates or symptoms at follow‐up.
Article
Full-text available
Aim/Purpose The purpose of this paper is to introduce, describe, and document the methods involved in the preparation of a mindset intervention built into a freshmen development course, and established after years of longitudinal research, that is designed to have a positive impact on the outlook, achievement, and persistence of first generation and under-prepared students. Background A number of studies conducted in the past fifteen years have concluded that grit, the persistence and perseverance to achieve goals, and growth mindset, the belief that skills and intelligence can be developed, are positive predictors of achievement; however, little focus has been placed on the implications at institutions purposed to educate minorities, first generation college students, and learners from diminished socio-economic backgrounds. Methodology A series of models were created, custom self-assessment scales designed, and a lesson plan prepared purposed to deliver a mindset intervention to edify students about and change perceptions of grit, locus of control/self-efficacy, growth mindset, and goal setting. The mindset intervention, as presented in this paper, was delivered as part of a pilot implementation to students enrolled in a freshmen professional development course at a Mid-Atlantic HBCU in the Fall of 2019. Contribution This qualitative paper documents an ongoing initiative while providing a workable template for the design and delivery of a mindset intervention that is believed will be highly effective with first generation and socio-economically disadvantaged learners. It represents the third paper in a five paper series. Findings Prior research conducted by the authors shed light on the need to explore non-cognitive factors that may affect student performance such as grit, mindset, engagement, self-efficacy, and goal setting. The authors postulate that a carefully crafted mindset intervention delivered to freshmen students from traditionally underserved populations attending a minority serving institution in the mid-atlantic region of the United States will yield positive outcomes in terms of student success. Recommendations for Practitioners As part of a commitment to positive student outcomes, faculty and administrators in higher education must be constantly exploring factors that may, or may not, impact student success. Recommendation for Researchers Research is needed that explores elements that may help to contribute to the success of under prepared college students, in particular those who are from low income, first generation, and minority groups Impact on Society Since, mindset interventions have been shown to be particularly effective with underserved students, it stands to reason that they should be adopted widely, and be effective at delivering positive outcomes, at HBCUs Future Research The authors have introduced the mindset intervention with freshmen business students enrolled in a required professional development course. Results of the self-assessments and reflection questions are being collected and coded. Additionally, students are being administered a survey designed to measure the perceived efficacy of the initiative.
Article
Full-text available
Mindset theory predicts that a growth mindset can substantially improve children’s resilience to failure and enhance important outcomes such as school grades. We tested these predictions in a series of studies of 9–13-year-old Chinese children (n = 624). Study 1 closely replicated Mueller and Dweck (1998). Growth mindset manipulation was associated with performance on a moderate difficulty postfailure test (p = .049), but not with any of the 8 motivation and attribution measures used by Mueller and Dweck (1998): mean p = .48. Studies 2 and 3 included an active control to distinguish effects of mindset from other aspects of the manipulation, and included a challenging test. No effect of the classic growth mindset manipulation was found for either moderate or more difficult material in either Study 2 or Study 3 (ps = .189 to .974). Compatible with these null results, children’s mindsets were unrelated to resilience to failure for either outcome measure (ps = .673 to .888). The sole exception was a significant effect in the reverse direction to prediction found in Study 2 for resilience on more difficult material (p = .007). Finally, in 2 studies relating mindset to grades across a semester in school, the predicted association of growth mindset with improved grades was not supported. Neither was there any association of children’s mindsets with their grades at the start of the semester. Beliefs about the malleability of basic ability may not be related to resilience to failure or progress in school.
Article
Full-text available
This study first aims to investigate the role of students' implicit theories on their two grit facets (perseverance of effort [PE], consistency of interest [CI]) and on their achievement goals, learning motivation, and achievement in the context of a challenging academic task. Secondly, the study examines whether PE and CI are related to students' achievement goals, learning motivation and achievement. We surveyed 1215 students from the upper secondary school level with a mean age of 17.5 years, who completed a compulsory academic certificate paper over approximately one school year. The results of this study reveal that a more incremental theory was positively correlated with students' PE and CI. Furthermore, an incremental theory supports adaptive motivational patterns, such as stronger learning goals and higher intrinsic motivation, through which implicit theories are weakly related to academic achievement. This study finds evidence that PE and CI exhibit different motivational patterns. Whereas PE is positively correlated with mastery goals, performance-approach goals, and intrinsic motivation, CI demonstrates negative correlations with performance-approach, performance-avoidance goals and extrinsic motivation. In addition, PE is only weakly and CI not at all correlated with academic achievement through more adaptive learning goals and intrinsic motivation. Overall, the results confirm the significance of implicit theories for adaptive motivational patterns in the context of an educational achievement task. Finally, this study supports the claim that PE and CI should be treated separately due to their different correlational paths with motivational variables.
Article
Full-text available
The present research looked at the importance of the concept of grit in University students based on a mixed-method approach. Study 1 comprised 440 University students. All were given the Grit Scale, the Perceived Stress Scale, the short Warwick–Edinburgh Mental Well-being Scale, the Office of National Statistics Well-being items and the Self-Control Scale. Levels of grit were significantly higher in female students, older students and postgraduates. Grit correlated highest with self-control. Study 2 looked at 340 University students. In addition to measuring self-control, mental well-being and grit, measures of resilience and mindsets were also added. A construct validity test of the Grit Scale showed that high grit scorers had significantly higher levels of self-control and mental well-being, were more resilient and were more likely to have a more growth oriented mindset. Grit varies with age and is most closely associated with the concept of self-control. The third study was a qualitative investigation with 10 successful graduates. Semi-structured interviews were coded using thematic analysis. Three broad themes emerged. The first, Passion and Perseverance, included themes of having short and long terms goals, resilience, dedication, and endurance. The second, Self-Control, included time management, self-awareness, prioritizing tasks and knowing strengths and weaknesses. The third theme identified was Positive Mindsets. This included having a positive attitude toward learning, the importance of feedback and constructive criticism and that success is not materialistic. The qualitative research has helped “unpack” concepts from the grit research and may enable University tutors to guide students better. Though these studies were only conducted in one English University, they have been stepping stones in our quest to discover what are the most important factors in determining student academic success? The development and piloting of our new Uni-Stride Scale, is the next step in this process.
Article
Full-text available
Background: Single-session interventions (SSIs) show promise in the prevention and treatment of youth psychopathology, carrying potential to improve the scalability and accessibility of youth psychological services. However, existing SSIs have conferred greater benefits for youths with anxiety, compared to depression or comorbid problems, and their effects have generally waned over time - particularly for follow-ups exceeding 3 months. Method: To help address these discrepancies, we tested whether a novel SSI teaching growth mindset of personality (the belief that personality is malleable) could reduce depression and anxiety and strengthen perceived control in high-risk adolescents (N = 96, ages 12-15). At baseline, youths were randomized to receive a 30-min, computer-guided growth mindset intervention or a supportive-therapy control. Youths and parents reported youth anxiety and depressive symptoms, and youths reported their levels of perceived control, at baseline and across a 9-month follow-up period. Results: Compared to the control program, the mindset intervention led to significantly greater improvements in parent-reported youth depression (d = .60) and anxiety (d = .28), youth-reported youth depression (d = .32), and youth-reported perceived behavioral control (d = .29) by 9-month follow-up. Intervention effects were nonsignificant for youth-reported anxiety, although 9-month effect sizes reached the small-to-medium range (d = .33). Intervention group youths also experienced more rapid improvements in parent-reported depression, youth-reported depression, and perceived behavioral control across the follow-up period, compared to control group youths. Conclusions: Findings suggest a promising, scalable SSI for reducing internalizing distress in high-risk adolescents. Clinical trial registration number: NCT03132298.
Article
Full-text available
Grit, growth mindset, ethnic identity, and other group orientation are four psychosocial variables that have been associated with academic achievement in adolescent populations. In a sample of 105 high achieving African American high school students (cumulative grade point average [GPA] > 3.0), we examined whether these four psychosocial variables contributed to the achievement of high achieving African Americans beyond the contribution of socioeconomic status (SES) and other demographic variables. Results indicated that the psychosocial variables were not significant predictors of academic achievement for the high achieving African American students in this sample. However, SES was a significant predictor of the academic achievement with a medium effect size. These findings suggest that interventions focused on grit, growth mindset, ethnic identity, and other group orientation may not be as effective as hypothesized.
Article
Full-text available
The present study examined the effectiveness of a Growth Mindset intervention based on Dweck et al.'s (1995) theory in the Hungarian educational context. A cluster randomized controlled trial classroom experiment was carried out within the framework of a train-the-trainer intervention among 55 Hungarian 10th grade students with high Grade Point Average (GPA). The results suggest that students' IQ and personality mindset beliefs were more incremental in the intervention group than in the control group 3 weeks after the intervention. Furthermore, compared to both the baseline measure and the control group, students' amotivation decreased. However, no intrinsic and extrinsic motivation change was found. Students with low grit scores reported lower amotivation following the intervention. However, in the second follow-up measurement—the end of the semester—all positive changes disappeared; and students' GPA did not change compared to the previous semester. These results show that mindset beliefs are temporarily malleable and in given circumstances, they can change back to their pre-intervention state. The potential explanation is discussed in the light of previous mindset intervention studies and recent findings on wise social psychological interventions.
Article
Mind-sets (aka implicit theories) are beliefs about the nature of human attributes (e.g., intelligence). The theory holds that individuals with growth mind-sets (beliefs that attributes are malleable with effort) enjoy many positive outcomes—including higher academic achievement—while their peers who have fixed mind-sets experience negative outcomes. Given this relationship, interventions designed to increase students’ growth mind-sets—thereby increasing their academic achievement—have been implemented in schools around the world. In our first meta-analysis (k = 273, N = 365,915), we examined the strength of the relationship between mind-set and academic achievement and potential moderating factors. In our second meta-analysis (k = 43, N = 57,155), we examined the effectiveness of mind-set interventions on academic achievement and potential moderating factors. Overall effects were weak for both meta-analyses. However, some results supported specific tenets of the theory, namely, that students with low socioeconomic status or who are academically at risk might benefit from mind-set interventions.
Article
Background: Students living in rural areas of the United States exhibit lower levels of educational attainment than their suburban counterparts. Innovative interventions are needed to close this educational achievement gap. Aims: We investigated whether an online growth mindset intervention could be leveraged to promote academic outcomes. Sample: We tested the mindset intervention in a sample of 222 10th-grade adolescent girls (M age = 15.2; 38% White, 25% Black, 29% Hispanic) from four rural, low-income high schools in the Southeastern United States. Methods: We conducted a randomized controlled trial to test the efficacy of the growth mindset intervention, relative to a sexual health programme. We used random sampling and allocation procedures to assign girls to either the mindset intervention (n = 115) or an attention-matched control programme (n = 107). We assessed participants at pre-test, immediate post-test, and 4-month follow-up. Results: Relative to the control condition, students assigned to the mindset intervention reported stronger growth mindsets at immediate post-test and 4-month follow-up. Although the intervention did not have a total effect on academic attitudes or grades, it indirectly increased motivation to learn, learning efficacy and grades via the shifts in growth mindsets. Conclusions: Results indicate that this intervention is a promising method to encourage growth mindsets in rural adolescent girls.
Article
Implicit theories of intelligence have been proposed to predict a large number of different outcomes in education. The belief that intelligence is malleable (growth mindset) is supposed to lead to better academic achievement and students' mindset is therefore a potential target for interventions. The present study used a large sample of university applicants (N = 5653) taking a scholastic aptitude test to further examine the relationship between mindset and achievement in the academic domain. We found that results in the test were slightly negatively associated with growth mindset (r = − 0.03). Mindset showed no relationship with the number of test administrations participants signed up for and it did not predict change in the test results. The results show that the strength of the association between academic achievement and mindset might be weaker than previously thought.