ArticlePDF Available

40 Years of Tax Evasion Games: A Meta-Analysis

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

We collect individual participant data from 70 papers that use laboratory experiments to examine individual tax evasion behavior (or “Tax Evasion Games”), in order to use meta-analysis to estimate the impacts of different public policy, experimental design and individual level variables on tax evasion choices. Our results show that standard enforcement variables like audits (including audit rules) and fines perform differently on the extensive and intensive margins. We find that other fiscal variables like a flat tax system, tax rates, and tax amnesties have unambiguous negative impacts on tax compliance, and that specific features of the experimental setting, such as how subjects are directed to report income, or whether taxes are redistributed to the participants or to a real life public good, have significant impacts on tax compliance. Our results also indicate that the demographic characteristics of the subjects (e.g., gender, experimental income, occupation, risk attitude) affect compliance.
Content may be subject to copyright.
I
t R
p
f
IC
IC=ItR f[t(IR)]
IN
IN=ItR
EU (I) = pU(IC) + (1 p)U(IN)
E U(I)
p f
t
N= 8
e
e
e
e
0
10
20
30
40
50
Percent
0 .2 .4 .6 .8 1
Tax compliance
p= 0.211
p= 0.097
p= 0.359
p= 0.070
p= 0.836
p= 0.441
p= 0.064
∗ ∗∗ ∗∗∗
p= 0.247
p= 0.733
p= 0.177
p= 0.968
p= 0.931
p=
0.001 p < 0.001
p= 0.143
p= 0.583
p= 0.014
p= 0.285
∗ ∗∗ ∗∗∗
∗ ∗∗ ∗∗∗
p < 0.0001
p < 0.0001
r= 0.08
r= 0.20
N= 42,041
p= 0.01
p= 0.049
p= 0.015
p= 0.019
p= 0.019
N= 4
LOST OBTAINED
REFUSAL UNUSABLE
∗ ∗∗ ∗∗∗
∗ ∗∗ ∗∗∗
∗ ∗∗ ∗∗∗
∗ ∗∗ ∗∗∗
∗ ∗∗ ∗∗∗
∗ ∗∗ ∗∗∗
∗ ∗∗ ∗∗∗
∗ ∗∗ ∗∗∗
... Furthermore, Fig. 3 illustrates the distribution of individual compliance rates in year 0 when the audit rate is 0.2 along with the other three audit rates. The meta-analysis conducted by Alm and Malézieux (2021) utilizes data from 70 papers on tax compliance laboratory experiments published between 1978 and 2018. According to their findings, the average audit rate is 0.2 (with a standard deviation of 0.15), the average fine rate is 2.01 (1.47), the average tax rate is 0.34 (0.11), and the average compliance rate is 65% ( 41%). ...
... Figure 4 shows the compliance rates and the proportions of full compliers and full evaders obtained in several experimental studies. Out of 11 articles, we extract the results from experimental sessions relevant to the taxpayer's compliance decision He reports that as a weighted average, the audit rate is 0.23 (0.22), the fine rate is 2.14 (3.74), the tax rate is 0.35 (0.09), and the compliance rate is 59.1% (30.1%), which are similar to the results of the meta-analysis by Alm and Malézieux (2021). under confidentiality. ...
... 15 Considering the eight experimental studies from the fourth column to the last on the horizontal axis in the figure, the audit rates are set at 0.2 or more in their treatments (refer to Table 3 in the Appendix), so the resulting compliance rates in all of those studies except for Laury and Wallace (2005) are around two-thirds. Those results corroborate Alm and Malézieux (2021). However, those audit rates are much higher than the actual rates as these are estimated to be substantially smaller than 0.1 in real societies. ...
Article
Full-text available
This study examines the impact of public disclosure of tax information on tax compliance using simulation experiments utilizing agent-based modeling. We investigate two scenarios: (i) partial disclosure, in which tax returns and audit results are made public without taxpayer identities, and (ii) full disclosure, in which this information is publicized with taxpayer identities. Our simulation results reveal that in the partial disclosure scenario, the effect on tax compliance is contingent upon the social state of individual moral values regarding tax payment. Specifically, partial disclosure has a positive impact on tax compliance when taxpayers exhibit a relatively strong moral consciousness and a negative impact when their moral consciousness is weak. In the full disclosure scenario, when the moral consciousness of the population is sufficiently weak, the average reported income decreases despite the positive effect of the naming-and-shaming policy on tax compliance.
... We measured dishonesty using a tax evasion game ( Figure 2). Tax evasion games have frequently been used in economic studies, 30 including in combination with honesty oaths, 14 and have demonstrated external validity. 31 These games model real-life tax reporting: ...
... ). Based on previous research,14,30 we chose a charity recipient in order to model how taxes redistribute resources and wealth within a society. Participants were also provided with a slider scale that showed how much taxes they would need to pay depending on the income they reported. ...
... whereas a participant generating an actual income of £1.00 but reporting £0 received a bonus payment of £1.00 (1 − .35*0). There was therefore an incentive to underreport the actual income in order to evade higher taxes and keep more of the income -as typical for tax evasion games.30 After reporting their income, participants answered questions measuring their Honesty-Humility, general trust, socioeconomic status, and sleep quality, as well as demographic information including gender, age, and nationality.As a final step, they were debriefed.Tax Compliance. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Dishonest behaviors such as tax evasion impose significant societal costs. Ex-ante honesty oaths—commitments to honesty before action—have been proposed as useful interventions to counteract dishonest behavior, but the heterogeneity in findings across operationalizations calls their effectiveness into question. We tested 21 honesty oaths (including a baseline oath)—proposed, evaluated, and selected by 44 expert researchers—and a no-oath condition in a megastudy in which 21,506 UK and US participants played an incentivized tax evasion game. Of the 21 interventions, 10 significantly improved tax compliance by 4.5 to 8.5 percentage points, with the most successful nearly halving tax evasion. Limited evidence for moderators was found. Experts and laypeople failed to predict the most effective interventions, but experts’ predictions were more accurate. In conclusion, honesty oaths can be effective in curbing dishonesty but their effectiveness varies depending on content. These findings can help design impactful interventions to curb dishonesty.
... As Dodge (2020, p. 26) notes, "creative data collection is the core of white collar crime research." Creative data collection includes the use of innovative measures of white collar crime as well as the use of multiple methods of measurement (Alm, 2012;Alm & Malézieux, 2021;Elffers et al., 1992). In recognition of this call, we introduce the donation task as an example of a creative behavioral measure scholars can adapt to test emerging theories of white collar crime. ...
Article
Full-text available
To comprehensively grasp, predict, and address the complexity of white collar crime, robust measurement tools are imperative. The burgeoning interdisciplinary interest in this domain has spurred the development of diverse assessment tools aimed at capturing offending behavior and its proxies. In the present paper, we provide an overview of the strengths and weaknesses of several commonly employed measures, including official statistics, victimization surveys, self‐report surveys, and (quasi)experimental measures. Drawing insights from this review, we then introduce a novel behavioral measure: the donation method. Although the task was originally designed to examine behaviors of interest to behavioral economists, we carefully outline strategies to refine and implement the donation method to assess white collar crime, addressing challenges such as task suitability and controlling for respondent behavior. Additionally, we provide a sample task to illustrate how such a tool could be used within this field. We conclude by underlining the appeal of adopting innovative measurement tools like the donation method to advance our collective understanding of white collar crime. By advocating for its widespread adoption and providing guidance for its use, we endeavor to catalyze progress in the field, facilitating unique insights and, potentially, more efficacious strategies for confronting and surmounting the methodological obstacles inherent in studying this elusive form of criminal activity.
... This variable does not appear to be associated with the choice to evade (χ 2 , P r > 0.05) and, of course, has a positive correlation (0.2200) with the question about willingness to take a risk. This result is in line with previous literature (Alm & Malézieux, 2020). Other control questions • When asked "How well were you compensated for your time and effort in this experiment?" ...
Preprint
Understanding the factors influencing people’s choices in tax compliance decision-making is still important because tax evasion is a crucial issue for governments everywhere. This lab experiment investigates how social norms influence tax compliance behavior. We examine the effects of positive and negative empirical and normative expectations using the opinion-matching approach for measurement. According to our results, normative expectations—as opposed to empirical expectations—most strongly impact people’s behavior. Surprisingly, positive empirical messages may have a negative effect, increasing tax evasion. Furthering our understanding of the causes of tax evasion, we also include a norm-following task to assess participants’ propensity to adhere to norms. This study presents new viewpoints on tax compliance while replicating some established conclusions from previous research sheds new light on the interaction between tax compliance and social norms.
... We explored the extent to which economic inequality affects citizens' tendency to cheat, with emphasis on two interrelated dishonest behaviors: tax evasion and cheating to earn extra income. Although tax evasion games are common in the economic literature (Alm & Malézieux, 2021), none of these studies have varied economic inequality experimentally. Therefore, participants were exposed to a simulated society with either high or low economic inequality. ...
Article
Full-text available
Although the link between economic inequality and dishonest behavior is well-established in the literature, their causal relation is still unclear. Here we investigate the causal link between economic inequality and dishonest behavior in a simulated society with high or low economic inequality, while holding middle-class status of the participants constant across conditions. Study 1 (N = 479) focuses on tax evasion, showing that high (vs. low) economic inequality increases the tendency to not declare taxable income. Study 2 (N = 228) analyzes cheating behavior, showing that participants under high (vs. low) economic inequality cheat more to increase their income. Together, the studies provide experimental evidence that economic inequality plays a causal role in dishonest behaviors, such as tax evasion and cheating.
Article
Full-text available
En dépit de l’importance indéniable des recettes fiscales dans la constitution du budget de l’Etat et, par conséquent, pour le financement des projets d’utilité publique, un nombre de plus en plus élevé de contribuables en Côte d’Ivoire refusent de payer l’impôt en dissimulant leurs biens ou en les déclarant partiellement. La présente étude tente de comprendre cette situation en examinant l’incidence de la perception de la sanction encourue pour fraude ou évasion fiscale et de la perception de la justice sociale sur l’attitude à l’égard du paiement de l’impôt chez les chefs de ménage d’Abidjan. L’enquête porte sur un échantillon représentatif de 350 participants recrutés selon la technique de l’échantillonnage par quota. Les données sont recueillies grâce à un questionnaire et traitées à l’aide du test du « t de Student ». Les résultats obtenus corroborent nos hypothèses. D’une part, les chefs de ménage qui sous-estiment la sanction qu’ils encourent pour fraude ou évasion fiscale sont moins favorables au paiement de l’impôt que ceux qui surévaluent cette sanction. D’autre part, les chefs de ménage percevant une injustice sociale sont moins favorables au paiement de l’impôt que leurs pairs percevant une justice sociale. Ces résultats pourraient aider à l’élaboration de stratégies de sensibilisation plus efficaces pour favoriser la conformité fiscale volontaire. Despite the undeniable importance of tax revenues in the constitution of the state budget and, consequently, for the financing of public utility projects, an increasing number of taxpayers in Côte d'Ivoire refuse to pay tax by concealing their property or by partially declaring it. This study attempts to understand this situation by examining the impact of the perception of the penalty incurred for tax evasion or fraud and the perception of social justice on the attitude towards paying tax among heads of households in Abidjan. The survey is based on a representative sample of 350 participants recruited using the quota sampling technique. The data are collected through a questionnaire and processed using Student's t-test. The results obtained corroborate our hypotheses. On the one hand, the heads of households who underestimate the penalty they incur for tax fraud or evasion are less favorable to paying the tax than those who overestimate this penalty. On the other hand, the heads of households perceiving a social injustice are less favorable to the payment of taxes than their peers perceiving a social justice. These results may help in the development of more effective awareness-raising strategies to encourage voluntary tax compliance.
Article
Full-text available
Does giving taxpayers a voice over the destination of tax revenues lead to more honest income declarations? Previous experiments have shown that giving participants the opportunity to select the organization that receives their tax funds tends to increase tax compliance. The aim of this paper is to assess whether this increase in compliance is induced by the sole fact of giving subjects a choice-a "direct democracy effect". To that aim, we ask participants to a tax evasion game to choose, in a collective or individual choice setting, between two very similar organizations which provide the same social (ecological) benefits. We elicit compliance for both organizations before the choice is made so as to control for the counter-factual compliance decision. We find that democracy does not increase compliance, and even observe a slight negative effect-in particular for women. Our results confirm the existence of a commitment effect of democracy, leading to favor more the selected organization when it was actively chosen. The commitment effect of democracy is however not enough to overcome the decrease in the level of compliance. Thanks to response times data, we show that prior choice on similar options as compared to a purely random selection weakens the preference for honesty. One important field application of our results is that democracy in tax spending must offer real choices to tax payers to improve compliance.
Article
Full-text available
Using two earned income/tax declaration experimental designs we show that only partial liars are affected by a truth-telling oath, a non-price commitment device. Under oath, we see no change in the number of chronic liars and fewer partial liars. Rather than smoothly increasing their compliance, we also observe that partial liars who respond to the oath, respond by becoming fully honest under oath. Based on both response times data and the consistency of subjects when several compliance decisions are made in a row, we show that partial lying arises as the result of weak preferences towards profitable honesty. The oath only transforms people with weak preferences for lying into being committed to the truth.
Article
This article brings an important empirical contribution to the academic literature by examining whether gender differences in tax compliance are due to higher prosociality among women. We conducted a large cross-national tax compliance experiment carried out in different countries—Italy, UK, USA, Sweden, and Romania. We uncover that women declare a significantly higher percentage of their income than men in all five countries. While some scholars have argued that differences in honesty between men and women are mediated by prosociality, we find that women are not more prosocial than men in all countries and we do not find a mediating effect of prosocial behaviour on tax compliance. Though tax evasion is a form of dishonesty, the tax compliance experiment is quite different from an honesty experiment, which is certainly one explanation for the different results. We conclude that although differences in prosociality between men and women seem to be context-dependent, differences in tax compliance are indeed much more consistent.
Article
This paper reviews recent economic research in tax compliance and enforcement. After briefly laying out the economics of tax evasion, it focuses on recent empirical contributions. It first discusses what methodologies and data have facilitated these contributions, and then presents critical summaries of what has been learned. It discusses a promising new development—the analysis of randomized controlled trials mostly delivered via letters from the tax authority—and then reviews recent research using various methods about the impact of the principal enforcement tax policy instruments: audits, information reporting, and remittance regimes. I also explore several understudied issues worthy of more research attention. The paper closes by outlining a normative framework based on the behavioral response elasticities now being credibly estimated that allow one to assess whether a given enforcement intervention is worth doing. (JEL H26, H30)
Article
Private information is at the heart of many economic activities. For decades, economists have assumed that individuals are willing to misreport private information if this maximizes their material payoff. We combine data from 90 experimental studies in economics, psychology, and sociology, and show that, in fact, people lie surprisingly little. We then formalize a wide range of potential explanations for the observed behavior, identify testable predictions that can distinguish between the models, and conduct new experiments to do so. Our empirical evidence suggests that a preference for being seen as honest and a preference for being honest are the main motivations for truth‐telling.
Article
Drawing on a unique dataset of leaked customer lists from offshore financial institutions matched to administrative wealth records in Scandinavia, we show that offshore tax evasion is highly concentrated among the rich. The skewed distribution of offshore wealth implies high rates of tax evasion at the top: we find that the 0.01 percent richest households evade about 25 percent of their taxes. By contrast, tax evasion detected in stratified random tax audits is less than 5 percent throughout the distribution. Top wealth shares increase substantially when accounting for unreported assets, highlighting the importance of factoring in tax evasion to properly measure inequality. (JEL D31, H24, H26, K34)