ArticlePDF Available

Mirroring Maps and Action Representation through Embodied Interactions

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

We present a neural architecture aimed to reproduce the qualitative properties of the mirror neurons system which encodes neural representations of actions either performed or observed. Several biological researches have emphasized some of its important aspects, for instance, the tight coupling between the sensorimotor maps, the crucial role of timing (temporal information for encoding and detection), or the neurons connectivity. We attempt to model these in a network of spiking neurons to learn the accurate temporal relationships between the sensorimotor maps. After the learning, the neural connectivity efficiently induces functional capabilities in the whole network exhibiting statistics comparable to small-world networks (e.g., scale-free dynamics and hierarchical organization) similar with observed evidences in the mirror neurons system.
Content may be subject to copyright.
MIRRORING MAPS AND ACTIONS REPRESENTATION
THROUGH EMBODIED INTERACTIONS
ALEX PITTI
ERATO Asada project, JST, University of Tokyo
Tokyo, Japan
HASSAN ALIREZAI AND YASUO KUNIYOSHI
ERATO Asada project, JST, University of Tokyo
Tokyo, Japan
In this paper, we present a neural architecture aimed to reproduce the qualitative
properties of the mirror neurons system which encodes neural representations of actions
either performed or observed. Several biological researches have emphasized some of its
important aspects, for instance, the tight coupling between the sensorimotor maps, the
crucial role of timing (temporal information for encoding and detection), or the neurons
connectivity. We attempt to model these in a network of spiking neurons to learn the
accurate temporal relationships between the sensorimotor maps. After the learning, the
neural connectivity efficiently induces functional capabilities in the whole network
exhibiting statistics comparable to small-world networks (e.g., scale-free dynamics and
hierarchical organization) similar with observed evidences in the mirror neurons system.
1. Introduction
The discovery by Rizzolatti and his team of particular neurons triggering to
specific actions, whenever performed by the subject himself or observed from
someone else executing it [1], brings out the tight coupling that exists between
perception and action. This neural population –, coined by the term “mirror
neurons system” (MNS) and found in the premotor cortex,– is important since
it sheds light not only on how our actions are represented within the brain but
also how we understand those performed by others; action understanding is
hypothesized to be the first stage for infants to develop higher cognitive skills
such as social interaction and imitation [14, 15]. Recent observations of human
and monkey MNS have permitted to dress an overall good picture of its
characteristics and functionalities [1,2,14,17-19]. For instance, it fires robustly
with exact timing to executed and observed actions, even when the action
sequence's end is occluded. Nevertheless, despite the advances done, few is
Work supported by grant from ERATO Asada project, JST.
1
known about its underlying neural mechanisms and computational principles.
As these neurons represent a very small portion within a larger population of
broadly congruent neurons (10 to 30 percent only present effective visuo-motor
congruency), some researchers question about their real significance and
contest their importance [17, 18]: How such system might support any
functional integration and rely on so few neurons? The computational models
proposed recently for reproducing its qualitative properties (cf. [14]) never
address this issue. We propose to investigate it and present our hypothesis on
how the MNS could be organized to represent actions and to fulfill its
qualitative and quantitative properties.
Our idea is as follows. To rely on so few neurons, the MNS must be critically
organized as complex systems are (e.g., [6, 16, 20]). Efficient neural
connectivity might permit to have specific neurons to support the network's
functional integration e.g., for cross-modal associations. We introduce in the
first part our motives to develop such kind of neural network that can exhibit
the same functionality as the MNS. We show that its characteristics make it a
small-world network which are networks with exceptional structure (e.g., fault
tolerance, short path length). Using this network, we reproduce then the
Rizzolatti's experiment exhibiting the mirror neurons features to trigger either
during action execution and action observation (e.g., during grasping). We
show that the network generates similar characteristics with the MNS revealing
sensorimotor coupling and multi-modal integration (e.g., the re-enaction of one
modality from experiencing the other) relying on critical neurons.
2. Motivation
The particular organization of the MNS present some similarity with those of
complex systems which may permit to understand its functioning. For instance,
mirror neurons fire to either performed or observed action but with accurate
timing (e.g., firing only at the time-to-contact during grasping). We believe
that this temporal characteristic is important since it requires efficient
information propagation and therefore efficient neural connectivity to respond
robustly. However, the neurons firing at exact timing represent a minority
within the MNS: Gallese and Rizzolatti discovered that the MNS has an
atypical distribution [1] following roughly two classes labeled “strictly
congruent neurons” and “broadly congruent neurons” with respectively 1/3 and
2/3 ratio suggesting that actions are represented by few neurons ([17, 18]
report a 1/10 and 9/10 partition). Since timing is crucial, these neurons must be
efficiently connected within the MNS in order to trigger contingently.
Therefore, these critical neurons must somehow generalize the spatio-temporal
structure of one action sequence into its action primitives ([14]). Reversely,
2
their damages can cause the degradation of the network performance and its
functional integration (e.g, one hypothesis of the cause for autism [10]). In
addition, new evidences demonstrating that actions are represented both
spatially and temporally at different description levels [2] support once more
the hierarchical nature of the MNS organization. Altogether, these
considerations suggest us that, in order to exhibit pragmatic representations
implicit from which appear the tight links between perception and action, the
MNS should follow a complex systems architecture exhibiting (i) robust and
redundant information processing relying critically on time, (ii) asymmetric
density distribution of the neurons connectivity, and (iii) hierarchical and
distributed representations.
Those properties, summarized in table 1), are an hallmark of scale-free and
small-world networks (SWN) [6, 7, 16, 20]. These are types of graph with
characteristic nodes connectivity distribution that follow a power-law
distribution and present efficient information propagation and synchronization
at different time scales (i.e., scale-free dynamics [7, 16]). A majority of units
possesses short path lengths connections with their neighbors, forming a semi-
closed cluster, whereas a minority possesses long path lengths linking those
clusters to distant ones. These special neurons represent hub connectors that
link the “small-worlds” to each other. Information exchange is particularly fast
because of the hierarchical organization that combine centralization and
distribution making the network robust to fault tolerance [20]. The suppression
of an important number of provincial units might weakly affect the network
performance whereas the suppression of the most connected ones might
drastically affect it, making them critical.
Table 1. Qualitative and quantitative comparisons between the
properties of the mirror neuron systems and of small-world networks.
Mirror neurons system Small-world networks
Tight coupling between perception and
action: critical timing [1]
Critical timing
SWN relies on few but critical
units integrating globally the
local processes.
MNS distribution [1]:
60% “coarse” neurons
30% “accurate” neurons
Autism: no global integration of modal
processes [11]
“Hub Connectors”
Connectivity distribution of the
units in a SWN follow a power-
law curve.
SWN relies on few but critical
units integrating globally the
local processes.
Neural representation at different
description levels [2]
Scale-free dynamics
Information is represented in
hierarchies at multiple time
scales.
3
3. Experiments
3.1. The neural network model
We attempt to model the particular entanglement between perception and
action characteristic to the MNS for action representation (e.g., grasping a
cup). To this aim, we define two networks to receive respectively the visual
information from the camera and the somatosensory input from a haptic device
(the force feedback of our fingertips), see Fig. 1. Each neuron of the visual
map is associated to one pixel value. As the camera resolution is reduced to
60x90 pixels, 5400 neurons constitute the visual map. The somatosensory map
has for itself 1000 neurons, each ones associated to a spatial location of the
haptic device [5,9]. The neurons, all excitatory, are defined by the formal
model proposed by Izhikevich [11, 12] to which we add 2000 inhibitory
neurons in a separated hidden layer to stabilize the overall system. The initial
neural network is to a standard random graph so that the node connectivity has
a uniform distribution: each neuron, either excitatory or inhibitory, has 100
synaptic connections equally weighted to other ones randomly selected over
the whole network. Therefore, the neural pairs within the same map support
the intra-map information processing (specialization) whereas the distant
neural pairs support the inter-map information processing (integration). Before
learning, these neural associations don't correspond to any particular
sensorimotor patterns; the mechanism of spike timing-dependent plasticity
(STDP) [4] regulates then the learning by updating the synaptic weights.
Figure 1. Schematic of the experiment. The network receives the co-occurent visuo-tactile inputs
during grasping from the camera (bottom-right corner) and from the pressure sensitive device (in the
upper-left corner).
4
3.2. The learning procedure
The first stage consists of repeated grasp of the tactile device. During
execution, the neurons of each map learn the invariant contingent relations
between their siblings within the same map (intra-map specialization) and of
the other map (inter-map integration). During time, they assemble themselves
to form robust spatio-temporal clusters between the vision map and the
somatosensory map. The result is such that, after the learning period, if we
reproduce again the grasping sequence, the network anticipates this time the
exact time-to-contact: the specific somatosensory pattern is activated before
the effective contact within dozens of milliseconds in advance (see Fig. 2).
Figure 2. Neural dynamics of the visuo-tactile maps during physical interactions; the blue dots
represent the neurons spikes. In red (resp. in cyan) the synaptic activation from the neurons of the
vision map (resp. the somato map). The retina anticipates the perceptual stimulus in the
somatosensory map before the time-to-contact. At the time-to-contact, the somatosensory map
generates in return a global activation.
5
This phenomenon, described by Berthoz as “anticipated touch [14], shows
how the neurons of the visual map literally simulate the activity of the tactile
modality with precise timing. The tactile and the vision modalities get
intertwined reproducing the mirroring effect of the F5 area's neurons. It
follows that, when we effectively touch the device at the time-to-contact
(receiving tactile stimuli), it is then the tactile map that generates a global
activation in the whole network. The two modalities are such functionally
integrated that one perceptual stimulus can activate (or simulate) the modality
missing. Such case occurs for example when we grasp an object with closed
eyes and mentally reconstruct its shape from our touches (tactile vision).
Or, when observing someone else grasping, we mentally reconstruct the
respective tactile information (vision → tactile).
3.3. Functional comparison with the MNS
To fulfill the comparison with the MNS, we test the network's response in the
conditions of action observation i.e., we provide to the network only the visual
stimuli (see Fig. 3).
Figure 3. Neural activity during observing a grasping sequence (no tactile information received). At
the time-to-contact and during handling, the visual map, without tactile information, re-activates
nevertheless the somatosensory activity as during enaction.
6
Without tactile stimuli, the network nevertheless re-activates the same neural
pathways as during enaction (see Fig. 3) and reconstructs the missing modality
from only the visual information: the tactile information is perceived from the
visual information (red links) and fires back (cyan links). Their dynamics thus
reverberate each other in a coherent fashion through a resonant-like process at
precise timing. It assesses the tight coupling between the two modalities and
suggests that the network is efficiently organized.
4. Analyzing the dynamics of the neural network
We analyze the network statistics with respect to the neurons connectivity.
Fig. 4 a) presents the distribution of the neural groups size relative to their time
span. Fig. 4 b) displays the neurons density distribution relative to the number
of synaptic connections. In Fig. 4 c), we analyze the network tolerance when
confronted to an attack (pruning neurons). The first one displays the wide
distribution of the clusters temporal range whereas the second shows the
power-law distribution of the units connectivity. These two graphs explain how
actions are represented inside the network as spatio-temporal clusters at
multiple time scales and at different hierarchical levels.
Figure 4: Clusters statistics. Density distribution of the neurons connectivity ordered by their time
span (a) [resp. the longest path of cluster defined and their time span]. The density of the neurons
connectivity in (b) follows the characteristic power law curve typical of small-world networks. The
network forms scale-free dynamics. In c), we compare the network performance (firing rate of the
somato map) depending if we prune the neurons in a random sequential order (blue) or the most
connected neurons first (red line).
7
As we introduce it in section 2, these properties are those of small-world
networks [7] and match the MNS quantitative data separating the neurons into
two classes asymmetrically distributed, those broadly congruent, the majority,
and those strictly congruent, the minority. This functional architecture is
hypothesized to imply efficient interregional communication, enhanced signal
propagation speed, computational power, and synchronizability [3, 6, 7]. For
instance, the network performance –, computed as the firing rate in the somato
map normalized between a lower and a higher limit interval,– decrease linearly
when we suppress neurons selected in an aleatory order (Fig. 4-c, blue line)
whereas the performance falls drastically if we prune the most connected
neurons first (Fig. 4-c, red line): the suppression of 37.5% of neurons randomly
selected or of the 7% most connected ones achieve the same performance
score.
The power laws curve means that neurons in a small-world network are not
completely independent from each other, and a few ones dictate the action. In
our experiment, it means that the neural network has evolved into an efficient
system shaped by the synchronized and coordinated visuo-somatosensory
stimuli. The network produces different description levels of the action across
multiple time scales assembled dynamically into short and long range clusters.
These clusters are organized around neurons highly connected (Fig. 4 b) which
articulate the scale-free temporal binding between the short-range time scale of
the neurons (millisecond order) to the long-range time scale of the
“body” (hundred milliseconds to seconds order).
Precisely, some of the neurons are found critical within the network due to
their large number of connections. Thus, not all of them have the same
importance. These particular neurons direct the neural dynamics and sustain
the network functional capabilities. It is remarkable that the network functional
integrity relies on a relatively small population of neurons compared to its
dimension: lesser than 5 percents of the neurons in the network possess more
than ten synaptic connections which represent approximately 300 neurons. We
circled in red these critical neurons in figure 5 and in black, some clusters
connecting some of them during grasping. As it can be seen from the graph,
they follow the trends of the visuo-somatosensory patterns (blue and green
lines). They represent therefore the primitives on which the dynamics of the
network are articulated and show how actions can be represented as scale-free
dynamics.
8
Figure 5. Critical neurons. We circled in red the neurons with more than ten synaptic connections
during the period of tearing [taken from Fig. 4]. They are critical for the functional integrity of the
network on which the clusters rely on. We plot also some clusters passing by some of these neurons.
The blue and green lines plot their trends, the action primitives.
5. Discussion
In this paper, we suggest the hypothesis that the mirror neurons system is
organized critically as a complex network to represent actions. Our proposal is
supported by recent findings suggesting that action representation in the MNS
are modeled at different description levels with scale-free dynamics [1, 2, 17]
relying on very few neurons [18, 19]. The neural organization of our model has
a topological connectivity similar to scale-free networks [3, 6, 7] which
permits the cross-modal integration between vision and haptic information.
Although the network is highly robust against pruning, global integration is
however sensitive to the suppression of particular neurons only highly
integrated, one hypothesis suggested for autism [10]. We expect that such
neural mechanism might provide some principles to understand how cross-
modal integration occur for action representation and action understanding
during infant development [9, 15].
Acknowledgement
We would like to thank the JST ERATO project for the support to this
research and the anonymous reviewer for his helpful comments.
9
References
1. Rizzolatti, G., Craighero, L., The Mirror-Neuron System”, Annu. Rev.
Neurosci. , 27:169–92, 2004.
2. Lestou, V., Pollick, F., Kourtzi, Z., “Neural substrates for action
understanding at different description levels in the human brain”, Journal of
Cognitive Neuroscience 20 (2), 324–341, 2008.
3. Bassett, D., Bullmore, E., “Small-world brain networks”, The Neuroscientist
12 (6), 512–523, 2006.
4. Abbott, L.F. and Nelson, S.B., "Synaptic plasticity: taming the beast", Nature
neuroscience, (3), pp. 1178—1182, 2000.
5. Pitti, A. Alirezaei, H. and Kuniyoshi, Y., “Cross-modal and Scale-free
Action Representations through Enaction”, Neural Networks (in press).
6. Watts, D., Strogatz, S., Collective dynamics of ’small-world’ networks.
Nature 393, 440–442, 1998.
7. Buzsaki, G., Rhythms of the Brain. Oxford University Press, 2006.
8. Alirezaei, H., Nagakubo, A., Kuniyoshi, Y., A highly stretchable tactile
sensor skin for smooth surfaced humanoids. IEEE-RAS 7th Intl. Conf. on
Humanoid Robots, 512–523, 2007b.
9. Rochat, P., Five levels of self-awareness as they unfold early in life.
Consciousness and Cognition 12, 717–731, 2003.
10. Just, M.A., Cherkassky, V.L., Keller, T.A., Kana, R.K. and Minshew, N.J.,
"Functional and anatomical cortical underconnectivity in autism: evidence
from an FMRI study of an executive function task and corpus callosum
morphometry", Cerebral Cortex, 17:4, p. 951-961, 2007.
11. Izhikevich, E., Gally, A.J. and Edelman, G.M., "Spike-timing Dynamics of
Neuronal Groups", Cerebral Cortex, 14, p. 933-944, 2004.
12. Izhikevich, E., "Polychronization: Computation With Spikes", Neural
Computation, 18, p. 245-282, 2006.
13. Berthoz, A., The Brain's Sense of Movement, Harvard University Press,
2000.
14. Oztop, E., Kawato, M. and Arbib, M., "Mirror neurons and imitation: A
computationally guided review", Neural Network, 2126, pp. 1-18, 2006.
15. Zukow-Goldring, "Assisted imitation: affordances, effectivities, and the
mirror system in early language development", from Action to Language via
the Mirror Neuron System, M.A. Arbib, 2005.
16. Barabási, A.-L. and Albert, R., "Emergence of scaling in random network",
Science, 286:509-512, 1999.
17. Chong, T. Cunnington, R. Williams, M. Kanwisher, N. and Mattingley, J.
"fMRI Adaptation Reveals Mirror Neurons in Human Inferior Parietal
Cortex" Current Biology 18, 1576-1580, 2008.
18. Dinstein, I., "Human Cortex: Reflections of Mirror Neurons", Current
Biology 18, R956-R959, 2008.
19. Dinstein, I., Gardner, J. Jazayeri, M. Heeger, D., "Executed and Observed
Movements Have Different Distributed Representations in Human aIPS", The
Journal of Neuroscience, 28(44):11231-11239 ,2008.
20. Albert, R. Jeong, H. and Barabási, A.-L., "Error and attack tolerance of
complex networks", Nature 206, 2000; p. 378–382. Erratum: Nature 409; p.
542, 2001.
10
... Reversely, how structural dysfunctioning in parietal-motor connections are linked to functional impairments as it is hypothesized to occur for autism? This chapter will present works on the modeling of the functional and structural organization of the mirror neurons system [Pitti et al., 2008], its functioning for representating oneself actions and for understanding those of others [Pitti et al., 2009a], its possible development during infancy for constructing infant's agency [Pitti et al., 2009b] and infant's body image [Pitti et al., 2010]. ...
... We organize this chapter as follows. We will present first how one efficient neural organization can emerge from mostly unstructured neural networks receiving bodily coherent inputs from different modalities with a timely-based reinforcement learning [Pitti et al., 2008]. The biologically plausible mechanism of Spike Timing-Dependent Plasticity rules out the contingency detection between neurons so that robust clusters distributed among the sensorimotor networks can assemble themselves during embodied interactions within the environment [Pitti et al., 2009a]. ...
Article
Full-text available
This chapter reviews several computational models of the ontogenetic and epigenetic mechanisms that contribute to the construction and to the functioning of the shared sensory-motor circuits in the parieto-motor cortex. The primary role of these shared circuits, which include the so-called mirror neurons system, is found to transform the sensorimotor activity to deal with the real-time interaction in the environment. However, many evidences indicate that their role also pervade to social cognition, which makes them particularly important to study regarding their contributions to infant’s motor and social development. We suggest that the special organization of the shared maps in the parieto-motor cortex that are capable to integrate multiple modalities and to avoid timing discrepancies, may present the characteristics of complex networks. Our simulations comfort the hypothesis that (1) the functional integration realized in those networks may be done by the reinforcement learning mechanism of spike timing-dependent plasticity and that (2) its regulation role may permit to understand the functions that sustain the neural representation of the bodily self –, like self-perception and body representation,– and of inter-subjectivity, like action understanding and self-other differentiation. Finally, these computational considerations may furnish some arguments for a decentralized (non motor-centric) view of the brain advantageously organized critically to allow functional integration within these circuits. Besides, any ontogenetic or epigenetic dysfunctions at one of these different stages could have dramatic consequence to later cognitive development as it is hypothesized to occur for autism. Keywords: spiking neural networks, mirror neurons, sensorimotor integration, self-other representation
Article
Full-text available
How similar are the representations of executed and observed hand movements in the human brain? We used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and multivariate pattern classification analysis to compare spatial distributions of cortical activity in response to several observed and executed movements. Subjects played the rock-paper-scissors game against a videotaped opponent, freely choosing their movement on each trial and observing the opponent's hand movement after a short delay. The identities of executed movements were correctly classified from fMRI responses in several areas of motor cortex, observed movements were classified from responses in visual cortex, and both observed and executed movements were classified from responses in either left or right anterior intraparietal sulcus (aIPS). We interpret above chance classification as evidence for reproducible, distributed patterns of cortical activity that were unique for execution and/or observation of each movement. Responses in aIPS enabled accurate classification of movement identity within each modality (visual or motor), but did not enable accurate classification across modalities (i.e., decoding observed movements from a classifier trained on executed movements and vice versa). These results support theories regarding the central role of aIPS in the perception and execution of movements. However, the spatial pattern of activity for a particular observed movement was distinctly different from that for the same movement when executed, suggesting that observed and executed movements are mostly represented by distinctly different subpopulations of neurons in aIPS.
Article
Introduction: Rizzolatti and Arbib (1998) argue in their exposition of the Mirror System Hypothesis that brain mechanisms underlying human language abilities evolved from our non-human primate ancestors' ability to link self-generated actions and similar actions of others (see Arbib, Chapter 1, this volume). On this view, communicative gestures emerged eventually from a shared understanding that actions one makes oneself are indeed like those made by conspecifics. Thus, what the self knows can be enriched by an understanding of the actions and aims of others, and vice versa. From this perspective, the origins of language reside in behaviors not originally related to communication. That is, this common understanding of action sequences may provide a “missing link” to language. In answering the question “What are the sources from outside the self that inform what the child knows?”, the basic idea is that negotiating a shared understanding of action grounds what individuals know in common, including foregrounding the body's part in detecting that the actions of the self are “like the other.” Given this footing, what then might the evolutionary path to language and the ontogeny of language in the child have in common? This perspective roots the source of the emergence of language in both as arising from perceiving and acting, leading to gesture, and eventually to speech. I report here on an ongoing research program designed to investigate how perceiving and acting inform achieving a consensus or common understanding of ongoing events hypothesized to underlie communicating with language.
Article
Systems as diverse as genetic networks or the World Wide Web are best described as networks with complex topology. A common property of many large networks is that the vertex connectivities follow a scale-free power-law distribution. This feature was found to be a consequence of two generic mech-anisms: (i) networks expand continuously by the addition of new vertices, and (ii) new vertices attach preferentially to sites that are already well connected. A model based on these two ingredients reproduces the observed stationary scale-free distributions, which indicates that the development of large networks is governed by robust self-organizing phenomena that go beyond the particulars of the individual systems.
Conference Paper
Stretchable tactile distribution sensors are significantly important in realizing natural and rich tactile interactions for next generation humanoids with soft and smooth surfaces. We have developed such a sensor which has no wiring in most of the sensing area and is based on inverse problem technique - EIT. At first, we show the ability of the sensor to detect novel and rich tactile interactions involving skin deformation such as pinching and rubbing. Furthermore, by using our newly developed conductive knitted fabric, we demonstrate how the sensor can be extremely useful for sensing pressure distributions as well as for covering complex 3D surfaces such as the face. Also, we show a successful implementation of the highly stretchable sensor over movable joints, the repeatability of the sensor value against joint movements and the detectability of external tactile stimuli for various joint angles are also demonstrated.
Article
The neuroscientist Alain Berthoz experimented on Russian astronauts in space to answer these questions: How does weightlessness affect motion? How are motion and three-dimensional space perceived? In this erudite and witty book, Berthoz describes how human beings on earth perceive and control bodily movement. Reviewing a wealth of research in neurophysiology and experimental psychology, he argues for a rethinking of the traditional separation between action and perception, and for the division of perception into five senses. In Berthoz's view, perception and cognition are inherently predictive, functioning to allow us to anticipate the consequences of current or potential actions. The brain acts like a simulator that is constantly inventing models to project onto the changing world, models that are corrected by steady, minute feedback from the world. We move in the direction we are looking, anticipate the trajectory of a falling ball, recover when we stumble, and continually update our own physical position, all thanks to this sense of movement. This interpretation of perception and action allows Berthoz, in The Brain's Sense of Movement , to focus on psychological phenomena largely ignored in standard texts: proprioception and kinaesthesis, the mechanisms that maintain balance and coordinate actions, and basic perceptual and memory processes involved in navigation. Table of Contents: Introduction I. Perception Is Simulated Action The Motor Theory of Perception The Concept of Acceptor of the Results of Action Bernstein's Comparator Memory Predicts the Consequences of Action Mental Nodes Mirror Neurons Simulation, Emulation, or Representation? II. The Sense of Movement:A Sixth Sense? Proprioception The Vestibulary System: An Inertial Center? The Functions of the Vestibular System Seeing Movement III. Building Coherence How Vision Detects Movement Visual Movement and Vestibular Receptors Am I in my Bed or Hanging from the Ceiling? The Coherence between Seeing and Hearing The Problem of the Coherence and Unity of Perception Autism: The Disintegration of Coherence? IV. Frames of Reference Personal Space and Extrapersonal Space Egocentric and Allocentric Frames of Reference Natural Frames of Reference Selecting Frames of Reference V. A Memory for Predicting Topographic Memory or Topokinetic Memory? The Neural Basis of Spatial Memory: The Role of the Hippocampus VI. Natural Movement Pioneers The Problem of Number of Degrees of Freedom The Invention of the Eye The Form of a Drawing Is Produced by the Law of Maximal Smoothness VII. Synergies and Strategies Vestibular Axon Branching and Gaze Stabilization The Baby Fish that Wanted to Swim Flat on Its Stomach The Neural Bases for Encoding Movement of the Arms Coordination of Synergies VIII. Capture The Toad's Decision The Art of Braking What If Newton Had Wanted to Catch the Apple? IX. The Look that Investigates the World Gaze Orientation "Go Where I'm Looking," not"Look Where I'm Going" Eye-to-Eye Contact Gaze and Emotion The Neural Basis of Gaze-Orienting Reactions X. Visual Exploration The Brain Is a Fiery Steed A Model of Perception-Action Relationships Imagined Movement and Actual Movement Dynamic Memory and Predictive Control of Movements Was Piaget Right? XI. Balance A Physiology of Reaction How to Make the University of Edinburgh Oscillate Toward a Projective Physiology XII. Adaptation Adaptation and Substitution The Rheumatologist and the Ophthalmologist The Role of Activity in Compensating for and Preventing Disorientation XIII. The Disoriented Brain: Illusions Are Solutions Illusion: The Best Possible Hypothesis Illusions Caused by Acceleration and Gravity Illusions of Movement of the Limbs Space and Motion Sickness A Few Other Illusions XIV. Architects Have Forgotten the Pleasure of Movement Conclusion: Toward a Tolerant Perception Notes Works Cited Credits Index Reviews of this book: This book is fascinating if one reads no more than the table of contents! With a jacket photo of semiweightless astronauts and chapter sections titled 'Am I in My Bed Hanging from the Ceiling,' 'The Art of Breaking,' 'What If Newton Had Wanted to Catch the Apple?,' and '"Go Where I'm Looking," Not "Look Where I'm Going"' among others, how could one not investigate further?...Anyone who has ever wondered, 'How did I catch that?,' 'How did I hit that?,' or 'How in the world did I get out of the way of that?!' will find this a good read. --L. A. Meserve, Choice
Article
Embodied action representation and action understanding are the first steps to understand what it means to communicate. We present a biologically plausible mechanism to the representation and the recognition of actions in a neural network with spiking neurons based on the learning mechanism of spike-timing-dependent plasticity (STDP). We show how grasping is represented through the multi-modal integration between the vision and tactile maps across multiple temporal scales. The network evolves into a small-world organization with scale-free dynamics promoting efficient inter-modal binding of the neural assemblies with accurate timing. Finally, it acquires the qualitative properties of the mirror neuron system to trigger an observed action performed by someone else.
Article
Claims to have identified mirror neurons in human cortex have been controversial. A recent study has applied an fMRI adaptation protocol to the problem and come up with novel evidence for the existence of movement-selective mirror neurons in human cortex.
Article
Mirror neurons, as originally described in the macaque, have two defining properties [1, 2]: They respond specifically to a particular action (e.g., bringing an object to the mouth), and they produce their action-specific responses independent of whether the monkey executes the action or passively observes a conspecific performing the same action. In humans, action observation and action execution engage a network of frontal, parietal, and temporal areas. However, it is unclear whether these responses reflect the activity of a single population that represents both observed and executed actions in a common neural code or the activity of distinct but overlapping populations of exclusively perceptual and motor neurons [3]. Here, we used fMRI adaptation to show that the right inferior parietal lobe (IPL) responds independently to specific actions regardless of whether they are observed or executed. Specifically, responses in the right IPL were attenuated when participants observed a recently executed action relative to one that had not previously been performed. This adaptation across action and perception demonstrates that the right IPL responds selectively to the motoric and perceptual representations of actions and is the first evidence for a neural response in humans that shows both defining properties of mirror neurons.
Article
Networks of coupled dynamical systems have been used to model biological oscillators, Josephson junction arrays, excitable media, neural networks, spatial games, genetic control networks and many other self-organizing systems. Ordinarily, the connection topology is assumed to be either completely regular or completely random. But many biological, technological and social networks lie somewhere between these two extremes. Here we explore simple models of networks that can be tuned through this middle ground: regular networks 'rewired' to introduce increasing amounts of disorder. We find that these systems can be highly clustered, like regular lattices, yet have small characteristic path lengths, like random graphs. We call them 'small-world' networks, by analogy with the small-world phenomenon (popularly known as six degrees of separation. The neural network of the worm Caenorhabditis elegans, the power grid of the western United States, and the collaboration graph of film actors are shown to be small-world networks. Models of dynamical systems with small-world coupling display enhanced signal-propagation speed, computational power, and synchronizability. In particular, infectious diseases spread more easily in small-world networks than in regular lattices.