ArticlePDF Available

Structure, developmental course, and correlates of children's anxiety disorder-related behavior in a Hellenic community sample

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

The generality of the DSM-IV diagnostic structure for children's anxiety disorders, as measured by the Spence Children's Anxiety Scale (SCAS) was investigated with a Greek-language version of the scale. An exploratory factor analysis produced a six-factor solution in general accord with the DSM-IV-based theoretical structure of responding. However, a generalized anxiety factor incorporated three unexpected items interpreted as representing excessive worry, including two items intended to measure obsessions, raising the question of children's ability to discriminate the intrusiveness of vexatious cognition. Anxiety scores were negatively correlated with school adjustment and performance, and decreased with age, with the exception of social phobia scores, which increased. Anxiety scores were substantially higher than those observed in most cultures, particularly on social phobia and compulsive behavior subscales. Hellenic children might regard compulsive behaviors as more socially acceptable than other anxiety disorder-related behaviors, whereas higher overall anxiety scores appear to be related to socio-economic circumstances.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Structure, developmental course, and correlates of
children’s anxiety disorder-related behavior in
a Hellenic community sample
Robert C. Mellon
a,
*, Adrianos G. Moutavelis
b
a
Department of Psychology, Panteion University of Social and Political Sciences,
136 Syngrou Avenue, 17671 Athens, Greece
b
National and Kapodistriako University of Athens, Greece
Received 17 August 2005; received in revised form 17 February 2006; accepted 20 March 2006
Abstract
The generality of the DSM-IV diagnostic structure for children’s anxiety disorders, as measured by the
Spence Children’s Anxiety Scale (SCAS) was investigated with a Greek-language version of the scale. An
exploratory factor analysis produced a six-factor solution in general accord with the DSM-IV-based
theoretical structure of responding. However, a generalized anxiety factor incorporated three unexpected
items interpreted as representing excessive worry, including two items intended to measure obsessions,
raising the question of children’s ability to discriminate the intrusiveness of vexatious cognition. Anxiety
scores were negatively correlated with school adjustment and performance, and decreased with age, with the
exception of social phobia scores, which increased. Anxiety scores were substantially higher than those
observed in most cultures, particularly on social phobia and compulsive behavior subscales. Hellenic
children might regard compulsive behaviors as more socially acceptable than other anxiety disorder-related
behaviors, whereas higher overall anxiety scores appear to be related to socio-economic circumstances.
#2006 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
Keywords: Anxiety disorders; Assessment; Children; DSM-IV; Spence Children’s Anxiety Scale
In the Hellenic Republic, investigation of the correlates and determinants of anxiety disorders
has been limited by the absence of reliable and valid measures of the behaviors that compose
them. Moreover, clinicians working with Greek-speaking populations have not enjoyed the
benefits of scales known to facilitate the differential diagnosis of anxiety disorders. Recently,
however, progress has been made in the development of Greek-language measures of anxiety
Journal of Anxiety Disorders 21 (2007) 1–21
* Corresponding author. Tel.: +30 210 920 1709.
E-mail address: mellon@panteion.gr (R.C. Mellon).
0887-6185/$ – see front matter #2006 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2006.03.008
disorder-related behaviors, with the standardization of the Hellenic Fear Survey Schedule for
Adults [(FSS-GR; Mellon, 2000) based on the Fear Survey Schedule (Wolpe & Lang, 1977)] and
of the Hellenic Fear Survey Schedule for Children [(FSSC-GR; Mellon, Koliadis, &
Paraskevopoulos, 2004) based on the Fear Survey Schedule for Children-Revised (FSSC-R;
Ollendick, 1983)].
In addition to providing requisite tools for Greek-speaking researchers and clinicians, these
studies have advanced our understanding of cross-cultural similarities and differences in the
structure of fears. For example, while the psychometric properties and factor structure of
responding on the FSS-GR and the FSSC-GR were broadly similar those obtained abroad, factor
analyses of both adult and child responses in Greece revealed clear clustering of behaviors
related to agoraphobic and travel fears that had not been previously observed in community
samples. In addition, analyses of Hellenic children’s responses revealed a structured dimension
of school performance fears as distinct from fears of failure and criticism (the correlation
between subscale scores based on these factors was relatively weak) that had not been observed
in other cultures.
Self-reported fear levels on both the child and adult Hellenic scales were substantially
higher than those observed in most western countries. The meaning of these differences is not
yet clear. Perhaps Hellenic respondents were more forthcoming in reporting their fears, in
which case the agoraphobic dimension in adults and children and/or the school performance
fear dimension in children might exist as latent structures in data from other countries. On the
other hand, fears in Greece might have a distinctive structure rooted in local cultural
practices. For example, well-structured agoraphobic and travel fears might issue from
common Hellenic child-rearing practices that foster family unity by emphasizing the
unpredictability, uncontrollability and severity of dangers that exist outside the home. A
structured dimension of school performance fears might issue from a Hellenic education
system that harshly penalizes minor failures in the recitation of vast quantities of materials
memorized by rote, encouraging analogous didactic practices by parents who are justifiably
concerned about their children’s eventual access to tertiary education (see Benincasa, 1997;
Flouris, Calogiannakis-Hourdakis, Spiridakis, & Cambell, 1994;Mellon, 2000; Mellon et al.,
2004 for discussions).
The Hellenic fear studies have also replicated two common findings on gender differences and
on the developmental course of self-reported fears; on average, males report substantially lower
levels of fears than females, and the levels of factor analysis-based fear subscale scores remain
stable or decline during primary school—with the exception of social and school performance
fears, which increase with age. In Hellenic children, both fears of failure and criticism and school
performance fears increased substantially between the ages of 7 and 12 years; by adulthood,
social circumstances are the predominant objects of fear and dread.
While data from the FSSC-GR have provided useful information on the normative structure
and the developmental course of children’s fears, the FSSC-R is generally considered to be a
measure of specific fears and phobias that does not measure other important dimensions of
anxiety disorders (McCathie & Spence, 1991;Muris, Merkelbach, Ollendick, King, & Bogie,
2002;Perrin & Last, 1992;Stallings & March, 1995). Specifically, while subscale scores from the
FSSC-R discriminate among social and specific phobias (e.g., Weems, Silverman, Saavedra,
Pina, & White Lumpkin, 1999) they do not provide measures of certain problematic behaviors
essential to the diagnosis of obsessive-compulsive disorder, panic disorder, generalized anxiety
disorder, and separation anxiety disorder. The development of reliable and valid measures of
these characteristics would be of great value to Greek researchers and clinicians, and additionally
R.C. Mellon, A.G. Moutavelis / Journal of Anxiety Disorders 21 (2007) 1–212
would contribute to the emerging characterization of the structure of children’s anxiety disorder-
related behavior in the Hellenic Republic and across cultures.
In addition to providing an overall anxiety score, the Spence Children’s Anxiety Scale (SCAS;
Spence, 1997, 1998) was developed to measure specific characteristics of childhood anxiety
disorders in community samples in accord with the diagnostic criteria of the DSM-IV (APA,
1994). Subsets of the 38 anxiety disorder-related items of the SCAS are intended to measure the
frequencies of occurrence of the following clusters of anxiety disorder-related behaviors, as
defined by DSM-IV criteria: panic disorder/agoraphobia (e.g., ‘‘I suddenly feel as if I can’t
breathe when there is no reason for this’’); social phobia (e.g., ‘‘I worry what other people think
of me’’); separation anxiety disorder (e.g., ‘‘I feel scared if I have to sleep on my own’’);
generalized anxiety disorder (e.g., ‘‘I worry about things,’ ‘‘I feel afraid’’); obsessive-
compulsive disorder (e.g., ‘‘I have to do some things in just the right way to keep bad things from
happening’’); and physical injury fears, that is, fears elicited by objects and events that are among
the most common objects of specific phobias in children (e.g., ‘‘I am scared of dogs’’). Empirical
evidence indicates that the SCAS can discriminate between children who satisfy the diagnostic
criteria for specific DSM-IV anxiety disorders from those who do not (Muris, Schmidt, &
Merckelbach, 2000;Spence, 1998).
Accordingly, the present report concerns the development of a Hellenic version of the Spence
Children’s Anxiety Scale and the implications of the results of its administration with a large
community sample with regard to the structure, developmental course and correlates of anxiety
disorder-related behavior. Beyond investigating the internal consistency, test–retest reliability,
and age- and gender-related differences of children’s self-reported anxiety disorder-related
behavior on a Greek-language version of the SCAS (the Hellenic Spence Children’s Anxiety
Scale, or SCAS-GR), the present study provides data for the further exploration of common and
uncommon characteristics of the structure of anxiety disorders in children across cultures, via a
comparative analysis with findings from research with the SCAS and with Hellenic and foreign
versions of the FSSC-R. In addition, teacher’s reports of the children’s school performance and
adjustment in relation to children’s self-reported levels behaviors that characterize anxiety
disorders contribute to the data base concerning the functional significance of these disorders, as
well as providing an assessment of the convergent validity of scores on the Hellenic SCAS.
1. Method
1.1. Participants
Data were collected at 17 public schools throughout the five school districts of the Athens-
Piraeus area, home to nearly one third of the Hellenic Republic’s 11 million residents. Fifteen
hundred and twenty children (778 boys and 742 girls) participated this investigation, representing
1.96% of all children registered in the 4th, 5th, and 6th grades. Fourth graders (N= 496) were 9–
10 years of age, 5th graders (N= 439) were 10–11 years of age, and 6th graders (N= 585) were
11–12 years of age. Schools were chosen to represent a range of socio-economic catchment areas.
The children’s reports of their parents’ education levels were as follows: primary education, 8.5%
of fathers and 6.2% of mothers; gymnasium (junior high school), 14.6% of fathers and 11.6% of
mothers; lyceum (high school), 32.6% of fathers and 33.3% of mothers, and tertiary education,
44.3% of fathers and 41.9% of mothers. By their own report, 18% of the children’s mothers and
20% of their fathers were born in foreign countries, principally in Albania, and 12% of the
children themselves were foreign-born.
R.C. Mellon, A.G. Moutavelis / Journal of Anxiety Disorders 21 (2007) 1–21 3
1.2. Instruments
The children were administered the SCAS-GR, a 45-item adaptation of the 44-item SCAS.
The ordering of the original SCAS items was retained except for one additional item, which
appeared as item 45; this item tapped fears of elevators (see below). As opposed to the FSSC-R,
which measures the intensity of fear elicited by objects and events, the SCAS measures the
frequency of occurrence of behaviors that are diagnostic characteristics of anxiety disorders (see
McCathie & Spence, 1991, for discussion). Accordingly, children were asked to rate the
frequency with which they experience each of 39 clinically relevant behaviors on a 4-point scale:
never (0), sometimes (1), often (2) or always (3); thus higher scores represent higher frequencies
of anxiety disorder-related behaviors. Consistent with the SCAS, the SCAS-GR contained six
items describing positively regarded behaviors (such as item 31, ‘‘I feel happy’’); these items
were designed to reduce negative response bias. Responses to these items are usually not scored.
Also consistent with the SCAS, a final item requested the participants to name anything else that
they were ‘‘really afraid’’ of, and to rate the frequency of occurrence of this fear. Because of wide
variance in responding, children’s reports on this item were excluded from the total anxiety and
subscale scores. The instructions indicated that there were no right or wrong answers.
The children were also administered the Parental Consequences for Students’ Behavior scale
(PCSB; Mellon & Moutavelis, 2005), a 62-item self-report instrument designed to measure the
frequency with which their parents positively reinforce, negatively reinforce, punish and provide
no consequences for their studying behavior. Data from this newly developed instrument will not
be reported here.
In addition, the children completed a six-item demographics questionnaire concerning their
parents’ occupation, educational level, and country of origin, plus their own country of origin,
number of siblings, and birth order, as well as the physical proximity of their close relatives.
Finally, the children’s teachers completed a questionnaire for each child, which included six
demographic questions plus eight questions drawn from a Greek-language translation of the
Teacher’s Report Form (Achenbach, 1991). Four of the latter questions tapped the children’s
performance in the Greek language, in mathematics, in history and in physics, on a 5-point scale
ranging from ‘‘well below average’’ to ‘well above average.’ The other four items measured the
teachers’ judgments of the children’s work habits, conduct, effort and disposition, using a 7-point
scale ranging from ‘‘well below’’ to ‘‘well above’’ average relative to other children of the same age.
1.3. Procedure
1.3.1. Adaptation of the SCAS and pilot evaluation
The SCAS was adapted from English into Greek in accordance with guidelines developed by
the International Test Commission (van de Vijver & Hambleton, 1996). At the outset, the
instrument was examined for uncommon referents and inconsistencies with local conditions.
Two minor changes were made. On item 30, ‘‘I am afraid of being in crowded places like
shopping centers, the movies, buses, busy playgrounds,’’ ‘‘shopping center’’ was replaced with
‘‘open-air markets.’ Shopping centers are to be found in Greek cities, but the ubiquitous ‘‘laikoi
agores’ (from which the term ‘‘agoraphobia’’ is derived) provide more familiar scenes of
crowded, energetic trading. On item 39, ‘‘I am afraid of being in closed places, like tunnels and
small rooms,’’ ‘‘tunnels’’ was replaced with ‘‘small storage areas.’’ While tunnels are
encountered in the Hellenic Republic, they are rarely if ever long enough to give one the
impression of entrapment.
R.C. Mellon, A.G. Moutavelis / Journal of Anxiety Disorders 21 (2007) 1–214
A more substantial alteration concerned item 25 of the SCAS, ‘‘I am afraid of being in high
places or lifts (elevators).’’ This item is from the SCAS subscale for the assessment of specific
phobia; however, it appears to conflate fear of falling from heights, which is theoretically a
physical injury fear, with fear of entrapment, which is theoretically an agoraphobic fear. On the
FSSC-GR, fear of elevators was quite weakly related to a ‘‘Danger and death’’ factor, and loaded
on a factor that included fear of closed spaces and fear of dark rooms and closets. Accordingly,
the descriptor ‘‘elevators’’ was removed from item 25 and, as clinical experience and FSSC-GR
data indicate that fears of elevators are rather common in Hellenic children, ‘‘I am afraid of being
in elevators’’ was added as a 45th item.
After these changes, the scale was independently translated by the bilingual authors (one a
native speaker of English, the other a native speaker of Greek). The two translations were then
independently back-translated into English, in counterbalanced order, by four bilingual native
Greek speakers (one clinical psychologist and three primary school teachers). Minor differences
between the two translations were resolved by comparison of the accuracy of the eight back
translations in discussions among the six translators.
The resulting version of the SCAS-GR was administered to 66 children (20 fourth graders, 23
fifth graders, and 23 sixth graders). Items were read aloud by the native Greek-speaking author.
At the conclusion of the administration, the children were asked to describe what they had
understood each item to mean. It was the authors’ impression that the items were adequately
understood by the children, an impression that was supported by the measure of internal
consistency of responding on the 39 SCAS-GR anxiety disorder-related items (Cronbach’s
a= .86). Thus the SCAS-GR as used in the pilot evaluation was deemed appropriate for more
extensive investigation.
1.3.2. Principle investigation
Data were collected in 75 classroom groups of 12–28 children (mean class size was 20.3).
Instruments were administered in the following fixed order: SCAS-GR, PCSB, and demographic
questionnaire, with all items being read aloud by the native Greek-speaking author. The
children’s names did not appear on the instruments they completed; the children were identified
by a number code, and were assured that their answers would be treated confidentially. Teachers’
questionnaires were completed on the same day of administration. Data were analyzed using the
statistical package SPSS version 12.0.
1.3.3. Evaluation of temporal stability of scores
Test–retest reliability data were collected from a sub-sample of 121 children (4th graders,
N= 34; 5th graders, N= 41; 6th graders, N= 46). The second administration occurred exactly 21
days after the first administration. The test–retest sub-sample was similar to the full data set
regarding gender (60 boys and 61 girls) and country of birth (foreign born = 12, or approximately
10% of the sub-sample).
2. Results
2.1. Psychometric reliability assessment
As measured by Cronbach’s a, the internal consistency of the children’s responses on the 39
anxiety disorder-related items of the SCAS-GR was .90, which is similar to the response
homogeneity reported for samples of similar-aged children in Australian (a= .92; Spence, 1998),
R.C. Mellon, A.G. Moutavelis / Journal of Anxiety Disorders 21 (2007) 1–21 5
German (a= .92; Essau, Muris, & Ederer, 2002), Japanese (a=.88; Ishikawa, Oota, & Sakano,
2001, as cited in Essau et al., 2002) and South African (a= .92; Muris, Schmidt, Engelbrecht, &
Perold, 2002) samples.
The three-week test–retest reliability coefficient of total anxiety scores (i.e., sum of 39 anxiety
disorder-related items) on SCAS-GR was .83, which was higher, as would be expected, than the
6th-month test–retest reliability reported for an Australian sample (r= .60; Spence, 1997), and
similar to the two-week test–retest reliability of Hellenic children’s responses on the
considerably longer (80-item) FSSC-GR (r= .88; Mellon et al., 2004).
Separate reliability analyses for younger (4th grade) children and for foreign-born children
produced results similar to those of the full sample, suggesting that these groups did not have
particular difficulties understanding and responding to the SCAS-GR items. Internal consistency
for 4th graders was a= .90 (N= 496) and test–retest reliability was r= .80 (N= 34). Internal
consistency for foreign-born children was a= .88 (N= 179) and test–retest reliability was
r= .95; however, as noted above there were only 12 foreign-born participants in the test–retest
sub-sample.
2.2. Factor structure and psychometric properties of subscale scores
As the dimensions of anxiety disorders, the descriptive styles of respondents and their
tendencies to disclose anxiety disorder-related behavior are likely to differ somewhat from one
culture to another, the SCAS-GR is properly considered to be a new scale. Thus, an exploratory
rather than confirmatory strategy was employed in the investigation of the factor structure of
Hellenic children’s self-reported anxiety disorder-related behavior; this approach affords
comparison of cross-loading patterns.
A principal components analysis with varimax rotation produced seven factors with an
eigenvalue of greater than one. Examination of the scree plot revealed six to seven factors, but the
last factor of the seven-factor solution was not interpretable. Thus, the six-factor solution was
selected as providing the best conceptual fit of the data; this solution accounted for 42% of the
total variance in responding. In comparison, a six-factor PCA with varimax rotation with a
sample of Australian schoolchildren (Spence, 1998) accounted for 47% of the total variance.
The factor structure is presented in Table 1 along with the eigenvalues, the percentages of
variance accounted for, and measures of the internal consistency (Cronbach’s a) and temporal
stability (r
test–retest
) of subscales based on the six SCAS-GR factors. Of the 39 anxiety disorder-
related items, 32 had their highest factor loading on the factor predicted on the basis of
independent judges’ categorization of SCAS items in the initial development of the scale
(Spence, 1998). This compares favorably to the 32 SCAS items that loaded on their predicted
factors in the initial development of the scale; however, on the SCAS only two items cross-loaded
(i.e., loading of .35 on a second factor) whereas the SCAS-GR had four cross-loaded items.
Letter codes to the left of each item number indicate the DSM-IV anxiety disorder diagnostic
categories that the corresponding English-language items were intended to measure in the
original development of the SCAS. A majority of items in each of the six SCAS-GR factors
corresponded to the items of one of the six SCAS theoretical factors.
The psychometric properties of subscales based on the six SCAS-GR factors were quite
similar to those reported for corresponding subscales abroad. In two studies that have reported
measures of internal consistency of responding for school-age children on each subscale (Essau
et al., 2002; Spence, 1998) Cronbach’s afor SCAS subscales ranged between .57 and .82, with a
median of .72; in the present study, they ranged from .56 to .78, with a median of .76. As opposed
R.C. Mellon, A.G. Moutavelis / Journal of Anxiety Disorders 21 (2007) 1–216
R.C. Mellon, A.G. Moutavelis / Journal of Anxiety Disorders 21 (2007) 1–21 7
Table 1
Rotated component reliabilities and item loadings from the exploratory factor analysis
Component EVariance % ar
test-retest
I. Panic disorder and agoraphobia (8 items) 8.57 21.97 .78 .78
II. Generalized anxiety disorder (9 items) 2.20 5.64 .76 .69
III. Separation anxiety disorder (5 items) 1.71 4.39 .71 .78
IV. Physical injury fears (7 items) 1.54 3.94 .71 .79
V. Social phobia (6 items) 1.28 3.28 .71 .76
VI. Compulsive behavior (4 items) 1.10 2.71 .56 .75
Predicted category, item number and item SCAS-GR factor loadings (with highest
factor loadings in italics)
I PA II GA III SA IV PIF V SP VI CB
Items with highest loading for Factor I: Panic disorder and agoraphobia (PA)
PA (36) My heart suddenly starts to beat too quickly for no reason. .67 .21 .09 .03 .09 .08
PA (32) All of a sudden I feel really scared for no reason at all. .65 .24 .18 .10 .04 .00
PA (13) I suddenly feel as if I can’t breathe when there is no reason for this. .62 .20 .12 .04 .07 .06
PA (34) I suddenly become dizzy or faint when there is no reason for this. .61 .18 .02 .10 .09 .06
PA (21) I suddenly start to tremble or shake when there is no reason for this. .56 .23 .07 .14 .01 .07
PA (37) I worry that I will suddenly get a scared feeling when there is nothing to be afraid of. .48 .24 .31 .14 .10 .02
SA (16) I have trouble going to school in the mornings because I feel nervous or afraid. .46 .05 .13 .06 .32 .11
PA (30) I am afraid of being in crowded places, like markets, movies, buses, busy playgrounds. .37 .04 .16 .22 .32 .20
Items with highest loading for Factor II: Generalized anxiety disorder (GA)
OC (19) I can’t seem to get bad or silly thoughts out of my head. .21 .65 .08 .10 .10 .07
OC (41) I get bothered by bad or silly thoughts or pictures in my mind. .21 .59 .15 .07 .15 .05
GA (1) I worry about things. .14 .51 .11 .08 .22 .04
GA (22) I worry that something bad will happen to me. .32 .48 .09 .17 .10 .16
GA (24) When I have a problem, I feel shaky .29 .48 .05 .20 .16 .05
GA (20) When I have a problem, my heart beats really fast. .27 .42 .16 .04 .10 .16
GA (4) I feel afraid. .17 .41 .39 .27 .07 .00
SA (12) I worry that something awful will happen to my family. .22 .39 .12 .09 .13 .17
GA (3) When I have a problem, I get a funny feeling in my stomach. .20 .30 .23 .19 .16 .04
Items with highest loading for Factor III: Separation anxiety disorder (SA)
SA (5) I would feel afraid of being on my own at home. .10 .14 .67 .23 .08 .06
SA (8) I worry about being away from my parents. .17 .09 .64 .07 .13 .23
R.C. Mellon, A.G. Moutavelis / Journal of Anxiety Disorders 21 (2007) 1–218
Table 1 (Continued )
Predicted category, item number and item SCAS-GR factor loadings (with highest
factor loadings in italics)
I PA II GA III SA IV PIF V SP VI CB
SA (15) I feel scared to have to sleep on my own. .11 .21 .62 .17 .10 .10
PIF (2) I am scared of the dark. .03 .31 .52 .36 .04 .08
SA (44) I would feel scared if I had to stay away from home overnight. .16 .06 .48 .21 .18 .11
Items with highest loading for Factor IV: Physical injury fears (PIF)
PIF (25) I am scared of being in high places. .01 .24 .13 .61 .06 .07
PIF (18) I am scared of dogs. .03 .12 .06 .59 .02 .02
PIF (45) I am scared of being in lifts (elevators). .16 .01 .18 .57 .15 .17
PIF (33) I am scared of insects or spiders. .00 .09 .26 .57 .13 .02
PIF (23) I am scared of going to the doctor or dentist. .19 .07 .14 .46 .22 .12
PA (28) I feel scared if I have to travel in a car, or on a bus or train. .32 .16 .22 .41 .22 .09
PA (39) I am afraid of being in small closed places, like small rooms or small storage spaces. .21 .15 .38 .39 .16 .13
Items with highest loading for Factor V: Social phobia (SP)
SP (6) I feel scared when I have to take a test. .08 .24 .08 .15 .65 .04
SP (10) I worry that I will do badly at my school work. .08 .32 .02 .09 .61 .06
SP (35) I feel afraid if I have to talk in front of my class. .20 .21 .09 .19 .58 .07
SP (29) I worry what other people think of me. .15 .37 .06 .09 .53 .05
SP (7) I feel afraid to use public toilets or bathrooms. .11 .11 .19 .01 .51 .14
SP (9) I feel afraid that I will make a fool of myself in front of other people. .08 .38 .10 .18 .46 .07
Items with highest loading for Factor VI: Compulsive behavior (CB)
OC (40) I have to do some things over and over again (like washing my hands, cleaning or putting things in a certain order). .02 .12 .07 .08 .06 .67
OC (14) I have to keep checking that I have done things right (like the switch is off or the door is locked). .00 .09 .07 .14 .00 .66
OC (42) I have to do some things in just the right way to keep bad things from happening. .09 .18 .06 .12 .03 .58
OC (27) I have to think special thoughts to stop bad things from happening. .17 .21 .21 .04 .02 .54
to the previous studies, in which the lowest measures of internal consistency were reported for
physical injury fears factors, in the present study awas lowest for Factor VI (CB) which was
composed of only four items as opposed to six items in obsessive-compulsive disorder subscales
in previous studies; the SCAS-GR physical injury fears factor had seven items in comparison
with the five-item SCAS PIF subscale, and its internal consistency was correspondingly higher.
Temporal stability measures for all six SCAS-GR subscales were acceptable.
2.3. Total anxiety disorder-related behavior scores
The SCAS-GR total anxiety scores are presented in Table 2. The mean total anxiety score for
all participants, 42.3, was considerably higher than means obtained from Australian (34.1;
Spence, 1997), German and Japanese (22.9 and 22.4, respectively; Essau, Sakano, Ishikawa, &
Sasagawa, 2004) samples of children of similar ages. Although their statistical reliability cannot
be assessed, these differences are considerable even if one takes into account the fact that the
SCAS-GR has an additional item due to the splitting of item 25 into two items (the mean of mean
scores on items 25 and 45 was 1.11, yielding a corrected total anxiety score of 41.2). However,
similar total anxiety scores were reported for low, low-middle and middle socio-economic status
sub-samples in South African school-aged children (43.4; 46.4, and 45.1, respectively) although
not for a middle-high SES group (25.2; Muris, Schmidt, et al., 2002).
An examination of the data summarized in Table 2 indicates that girls, on average, had higher
total anxiety scores than boys, and that total anxiety scores tended to decrease with increases in
age, especially between the 5th and 6th grades. The reliabilities of these gender and age
differences were evaluated by a 2 (Gender) by 3 (grade in school) ANOVA on the total anxiety
scores. This analysis revealed significant main effects for gender [F(1, 1438) = 86.53, P<.001]
and grade [F(2, 1438) = 12.02, P<.001] with non-significant interaction. A Student–Newman–
Keuls post hoc analysis (a= .05) revealed that while total anxiety scores for 6th graders were
reliably lower than scores for 5th graders, the latter scores were not reliably lower than those of
4th graders.
2.4. Anxiety disorder subscale scores
In Table 3, the mean subscale scores and their standard deviations are presented for each age
group and gender. Age by gender ANOVAs revealed that girls’ scores were significantly higher
than boys’ scores on all subscales except compulsive behavior, where no gender differences were
found [PA, F(1, 1498) = 32.87, P<.001; GA, F(1, 1498) = 19.87, P<.001; SA, F(1,
1507) = 73.96, P<.001; PIF, F(1, 1501) = 123.28, P<.001; SP, F(1, 1506) = 64.57, P<.001;
CB, F(1, 1508) = 1.38, P= .24]. Significant age group effects were found for all six subscales
[PA, F(2, 1498) = 22.02, P<.001, Student–Newman–Keuls (a= .05), 4th >5th >6th; GA,
F(2, 1498) = 8.25, P<.001, 4th = 5th >6th; SA, F(2, 1507) = 20.23, P<.001, 4th = 5th >6th;
R.C. Mellon, A.G. Moutavelis / Journal of Anxiety Disorders 21 (2007) 1–21 9
Table 2
Means (and standard deviations) of SCAS-GR total anxiety scores by school grade and gender
Group All grades 4th grade 5th grade 6th grade
All participants 42.3 (17.0) 44.3 (18.1) 43.8 (17.4) 39.7 (15.4)
Boys 38.4 (15.9) 39.5 (15.7) 39.8 (17.0) 36.5 (15.0)
Girls 46.4 (17.2) 49.0 (19.1) 48.2 (16.9) 43.0 (15.1)
PIF, F(1, 1501) = 12.37, P<.001, 4th = 5th >6th; SP, F(2, 1506) = 6.65, P<.001,
4th <5th = 6th; CB, F(2, 1508) = 19.12, P<.001, 4th >5th >6th]. Thus, subscale scores
decreased with increases in age for all subscales except social phobia, for which scores increased
with age.
Because the subscales have different numbers of items, Table 3 also presents the mean item
scores for each subscale, along with their standard deviations, for each age group and gender, in
order to simplify comparisons of item endorsementacross the diagnostic dimensions. For all groups
except one, the ordering of sizes of mean item scores were the same: the lowest mean scores were
reported for the items of the panic disorder/agoraphobia subscale followed, in ascending order, by
mean item scores on physical injury fears, separation anxiety disorder, generalized anxiety
disorder, and social phobia, with the highest item means on the compulsive behavior subscale. The
exception occurred with the oldest group of children (6th grade) for which the item mean on
physical injury fears was slightly higher than that of separation anxiety disorder items.
For comparison purposes, Table 3 also presents the interpolated item means (and their
standard deviations) from the corresponding subscales of a sample of similar-aged Australian
schoolchildren (Spence, 1997). From this comparison it appears that the relative elevations of
scores observed with children in Hellenic primary schools were not evenly distributed across
anxiety disorder subscales. Specifically, while the mean item scores of Hellenic children were
only 5% higher than those of Australian children on the panic disorder/agoraphobia dimension,
they were between 17 and 24% higher on GA, SA, PIF and SP. Most strikingly, mean item scores
on the four-item SCAS-GR compulsive behavior subscale were 82% higher than mean item
scores on the six-item obsessive-compulsive disorder subscale of the SCAS.
The extraordinarily high scores on the items of the SCAS-GR compulsive behavior subscale,
together with the failure to observe a gender difference in self-disclosure on this subscale, raise
the possibility that for children in Hellenic primary schools, the behaviors reported on CB might
have been viewed as more socially acceptable or desirable relative to behaviors reported on other
scales. This possibility was examined with the use of the positively valenced filler items on the
scale. As noted above, the Spence scale contains six items such as ‘‘I am popular among other
R.C. Mellon, A.G. Moutavelis / Journal of Anxiety Disorders 21 (2007) 1–2110
Table 3
Means (and standard deviations) of SCAS-GR subscale scores and means (and standard deviations) ofitem scores for each
factor (in italics), by age and gender, together with mean item scores interpolated from Spence (1997)
Group PA GA SA PIF SP CB
All participants 4.2 (3.9) 10.8 (4.9) 5.1 (3.6) 6.9 (4.4) 8.4 (4.0) 7.3 (2.8)
.52 (.48)1.20 (.55)1.03 (.72).98 (.62)1.39 (.67)1.82 (.71)
Boys 3.7 (1.5) 10.2 (4.8) 4.4 (3.5) 5.8 (4.0) 7.6 (3.9) 7.2 (3.0)
.46 (.43)1.14 (.54).87 (.69).82 (.57)1.26 (.65)1.79 (.74)
Girls 4.7 (4.2) 11.4 (5.0) 5.9 (3.6) 8.1 (4.4) 9.2 (4.0) 7.4 (2.7)
.59 (.53)1.26 (.55)1.18 (.72)1.16 (.63)1.53 (.67)1.84 (.67)
4th grade 5.0 (4.5) 10.9 (5.2) 5.5 (3.8) 7.5 (4.6) 7.9 (3.1) 7.9 (2.9)
.61 (.56)1.21 (.57)1.11 (.75)1.07 (.66)1.31 (.65)1.96 (.71)
5th grade 4.3 (3.7) 11.4 (5.0) 5.7 (3.8) 7.1 (4.4) 8.4 (4.0) 7.2 (2.8)
.54 (.46)1.26 (.56)1.13 (.75)1.01 (.63)1.40 (.67)1.81 (.70)
6th grade 4.2 (3.9) 10.2 (4.5) 4.4 (3.3) 6.9 (4.4) 8.7 (4.1) 6.8 (2.8)
.43 (.41)1.13 (.50).89 (.65).90 (.57)1.46 (.68)1.70 (.69)
Spence (1997) .47 (.47)1.03 (.37).81 (.59).74 (.55)1.15 (.78)1.0 (.61)
kids my own age,’’ ‘‘I am a good person’’ and ‘‘I am proud of my school work,’’ that are intended
to reduce negative response bias. While these items are not normally scored, the sum of responses
on these items might be considered to provide a measure of children’s self-regard. As a subscale,
these items yielded a Cronbach’s aof .67 and a test–retest reliability of .64. As children with
higher levels of anxiety disorder-related behaviors tend to judge themselves as less able to control
their environments (Ollendick, 1983), self-regard and scores on anxiety disorder subscales would
be expected to correlate negatively.
Indeed, scores on five of the six SCAS-GR anxiety disorder subscales were negatively correlated
with self-regard scores [range: r=.18 (SA) to .29 (SP); all with P<.001]. The exception was
the compulsive behavior subscale, scores on which were positively correlated with self-regard
scores (r= .13, P<.001). The correlation coefficient was larger (r= .19) for boys alone.
2.5. Levels of anxiety-related behavior in relation to socio-economic indicators
In an effort to investigate the basis of the higher scores on the Hellenic SCAS in comparison
with SCAS scores abroad, the relation of anxiety scores to socio-economic status was
considered because, as noted above, in a previous investigation SCAS scores were considerably
higher in lower SES groups (Muris, Schmidt, et al., 2002; note, however, that there were not
only economic but also cultural differences between these groups). In the present study,
participants’ self-reports of their parents’ occupations proved to be too vague to reliably
categorize; thus, the parent’s levels of education were used as indicators of socio-economic
status. Despite the fact that by their children’s reports, more than 70% of mothers worked
outside the home, mothers’ education levels were not systematically related to their children’s
anxiety levels. However, fathers’ education levels (which might be more closely related, on
average, to familial socio-economic status) were systematically related their children’s anxiety
levels, as is shown in Table 4 .
In the Hellenic Republic, education is compulsory until the completion of the gymnasium
(roughly, junior high school). Because the number of fathers that had finished their studies at this
compulsory level was much smaller than the number who had finished secondary or tertiary
education, equal-sized random sub-samples were taken from the participants of the latter two
groups (N= 324 from each group); these sub-samples were matched to the compulsory-
education sub-sample with regard to grade level and gender distributions. An ANOVA indicated a
statistically significant main effect of fathers’ education level in relation to their children’s self-
reported total anxiety scores [F(2, 923) = 4.85; P<.01] with a Student–Newman–Keuls post hoc
analysis (a= .05) indicating that the anxiety levels of the children of fathers who had completed
R.C. Mellon, A.G. Moutavelis / Journal of Anxiety Disorders 21 (2007) 1–21 11
Table 4
Fathers’ education levels and children’s birthplace in relation to children’s mean levels of anxiety disorder-related
behavior (with their standard deviations)
Group Total anxiety PA GA SA PIF SP CB
Compulsory (N= 324) 44.5 (15.8) 4.5 (3.7) 11.2 (4.7) 5.5 (3.4) 7.3 (4.3) 8.8 (3.8) 7.3 (2.8)
Secondary
a
(N= 324) 41.8 (16.5) 4.1 (3.6) 11.0 (4.9) 5.0 (3.5) 6.5 (4.1) 8.3 (4.1) 7.3 (2.8)
Tertiary
a
(N= 324) 40.5 (16.6) 3.7 (3.6) 10.2 (4.7) 4.9 (3.7) 6.8 (4.4) 8.2 (4.2) 7.2 (2.9)
Foreign-born (N= 179) 46.2 (16.6) 4.8 (3.8) 11.6 (4.5) 5.4 (3.5) 8.5 (4.6) 9.0 (4.0) 7.3 (2.8)
Native-born
a
(N= 179) 41.6 (16.7) 3.8 (3.6) 10.8 (5.0) 5.2 (3.8) 6.6 (4.5) 8.1 (4.2) 7.4 (2.9)
a
Randomly selected sub-samples matched for distributions of school grade and gender.
only compulsory education were reliably higher than those of the children whose fathers had
completed secondary or tertiary education. A series of ANOVAS on the subscale scores indicated
moderate main effects of fathers’ education only for the panic disorder/agoraphobia [F(2,
962) = 3.87; P<.05] and the generalized anxiety disorder [F(2, 962) = 4.46; P<.05]
dimensions; scores on both subscales were reliably lower for participants whose fathers were
reported to have completed university or technical schools.
As a second socio-economic indicator, levels of anxiety disorder-related behaviors were
compared in foreign-born and native-born children. The birth countries of the foreign-born
participants in the sample are all sources of economic immigration to the Hellenic Republic;
thus a large majority of these participants are reasonably assumed to be the children of
relatively recent economic immigrants. Because the number of foreign-born children was much
smaller than the number of native-born children, an equal-sized comparison group of native-
born participants, matched for grade level and gender distributions, was formed by random
selection.
The results of this comparison are also presented in Table 4. On average, children assumed to
be the offspring of recent economic immigrants had higher total anxiety scores than native-born
children; an ANOVA indicated that this difference was statistically reliable [F(1, 357) = 6.52,
P<.01]. With the exception of the compulsive behavior scale, foreign-born subjects’ scores
were higher on all subscales; however, differences were statistically significant only for the panic
disorder/agoraphobia [F(1, 357) = 6.47, P<.01] and physical injury fears [F(1, 357) = 15.33,
P<.001] subscales.
2.6. Discriminate and convergent validity assessments
In order to discriminate between levels of behaviors associated with different anxiety
disorders, the intercorrelations between subscale scores should be substantially lower than the
correlation of scores on each subscale with total anxiety scores. Table 5 presents the
intercorrelations between subscales and, for comparison purposes, the correlation of scores on
each subscale with total anxiety scores. All subscale intercorrelations were substantially lower
than the respective correlations of scores on each subscale with the total anxiety score. The
SCAS-GR subscale intercorrelations were lower than 29 of the 30 corresponding intercorrela-
tions reported for German (Muris et al., 2000) and Australian (Spence, 1998) children of similar
ages. Range, median, and mean intercorrelations for the three samples were as follows: Hellenic,
.15–.63, .45, .40; German, .41–.69, .59, .57; Australian, .55–.91, .78, .76.
R.C. Mellon, A.G. Moutavelis / Journal of Anxiety Disorders 21 (2007) 1–2112
Table 5
Intercorrelations among subscale and total anxiety scores
Scales PA GA SA PIF SP CB Total
PA Panic/agoraphobia
GA Generalized anxiety .63
SA Separation anxiety .47 .47
PIF Physical injury fears .45 .45 .59
SP Social phobia .46 .54 .40 .46
CB Compulsive behavior .27 .28 .16 .15 .17
Total anxiety score .78 .82 .72 .75 .73 .42
Teachers’ evaluation of performance & adjustment .21 .14 .10 .11 .14 .09 .19
Note also that the correlations of scores on the compulsive behavior subscale with total
anxiety scores and with the other subscale scores were substantially lower than the corresponding
correlations among scores on other subscales; thus, compulsive behavior scores were the least
predictive of total anxiety as well as of levels of anxiety disorder-related behaviors as measured
by other SCAS-GR subscales.
Convergent validity was assessed by evaluation of the relation between teachers’ assessments
of their students’ school performance and adjustment and levels of behaviors related to anxiety
disorders. If the SCAS-GR measures levels of anxiety disorder-related behavior, which is
assumed to be incongruent with school performance and adjustment (e.g., Papay & Speilberger,
1986;Van Ameringen, Mancini, & Farvolden, 2003), these variables would be expected to
correlate negatively. Teachers’ evaluation of students’ school performance in four subjects and of
their school adjustment on four dimensions were highly intercorrelated; internal consistency
among the eight items as measured by Cronbach’s awas .94. Therefore, the eight items were
combined into a single global measure of school performance and adjustment. As presented in
the bottom row of Table 5, the teachers’ combined evaluations of the children’s school
performance and adjustment were indeed negatively correlated with the children’s self-reports of
the frequencies of occurrence of anxiety disorder-related behaviors, as measured by total anxiety
scores well as by scores on each of the six subscales. Correlation coefficient values were modest,
resembling those from a similar assessment of the relation between anxiety and school
performance levels with younger children (Papay & Speilberger, 1986); however, all correlations
were statistically significant (two-tailed, P<.001).
2.7. Most frequently endorsed anxiety disorder-related behaviors
To explore the most frequently endorsed anxiety disorder-related behaviors, the 10 SCAS-
GR items that most often evoked the evaluation ‘‘often’’ or ‘‘always’’ were tabulated and are
presented in Tab l e 6 . All four items of the compulsive behavior subscale were among the most
frequently endorsed items for all groupings of Hellenic children. Four items from the social
phobia subscale were also among the 10 most frequently endorsed items for all subjects
overall; the rankings of these items rose systematically with the children’s ages. [Although not
shown in Table 6, the two other SP items also showed age-related increases in endorsement
ranking: item 10 (‘‘I worry that I will do badly at my school work’’) was ranked 19th for 4th
graders, 17th for 5th graders, and 11th for 6th graders; similarly, item 35 (‘‘I feel afraid if I
have to talk in front of my class’’) rose from 30th for 4th graders to 25th for 5th graders and to
15th for 6th graders.]
A comparison of the ‘top 10’ SCAS-GR items with previously published data on the 10
most frequently endorsed items for both German and Japanese schoolchildren (Essau et al.,
2004) revealed four common items for Hellenic and German samples and five common items
for Hellenic and Japanese samples; German and Japanese samples also had five common
items. Only item 33 (PIF, ‘‘I am scared of insects or spiders’’) was among the most frequently
endorsed items for all three samples. Hellenic and German samples had two SP items and one
CB item in common among the 10 most frequently endorsed anxiety disorder-related
behaviors, and the Hellenic and Japanese samples had two other CB items, plus one other SP
item and one GA item in common. Although both Japanese and German samples had several
compulsive behavior and social phobia items among the most frequently endorsed items,
their ‘top 10’ lists were not dominated by these two categories as was true of the Hellenic
sample.
R.C. Mellon, A.G. Moutavelis / Journal of Anxiety Disorders 21 (2007) 1–21 13
3. Discussion
Overall, the present results might be viewed as providing strong support for the generality of
DSM-IV conceptualization of the structure of anxiety disorders, as measured by the Spence
Children’s Anxiety Scale, to a distinct Balkan and Mediterranean culture. Although an
exploratory rather than confirmatory strategy was pursued in the evaluation of the factor structure
of responding on the Hellenic SCAS, the most parsimonious solution attempted was remarkably
similar to that obtained with the original Australian sample, thus conforming well to the
diagnostic structure of children’s anxiety disorders as defined by the DSM-IV. The subscales
derived from the factor analysis were similar in both item content and in psychometric properties
to those obtained in a number of cultures.
In general, cross-cultural precedence exists for the SCAS-GR items that had their highest
factor loading on a non-predicted subscale and for items that cross-loaded on a second factor. For
example, while the item ‘‘I have trouble going to school in the morning because I feel nervous or
afraid’’ was theoretically a separation anxiety item, its highest SCAS-GR loading was on the
panic disorder/agoraphobia factor. This item had also failed to load on SA in Australian (Spence,
1998) and South African (Muris, Schmidt, et al., 2002) samples, loading instead on social phobia
(SP) factors; the present loading on SP was .32. The high loading of this item on PA is
understandable if one considers that an unwillingness to leave the home is characteristic of
R.C. Mellon, A.G. Moutavelis / Journal of Anxiety Disorders 21 (2007) 1–2114
Table 6
Ten most frequently endorsed (rated as occurring ‘‘often’’ or ‘‘always’’) anxiety disorder-related behaviors by gender and
age group (item ranking and, in italics, percent of children endorsing)
Subscale and item All Boys
only
Girls
only
4th
grade
5th
grade
6th
grade
(CB) I have to keep checking that I have done things
right (like the switch is off or the door is locked).
11 1 1 1 1
74.1 75.1 76.9 82.8 74.1 71.6
(SP) I feel afraid that I will make a fool of myself
in front of other people.
22 2 4 2 2
59.7 56.3 63.3 57.7 61.6 62.6
(CB) I have to do some things over and over again
(like washing my hands, cleaning or putting
things in a certain order).
33 3 3 4 3
56.7 56.3 60.0 59.9 57.4 53.5
(CB) I have to think special thoughts to
stop bad things from happening.
44 5 2 2 9
55.2 53.2 57.3 65.0 61.6 42.2
(CB) I have to do some things in just the right
way to keep bad things from happening.
55 9 6 8 5
48.8 51.6 48.8 49.7 46.2 49.9
(SP) I feel scared when I have to take a test. 6 11 6 15 5 4
46.0 37.1 55.4 38.8 47.8 50.8
(GA)When I have a problem, my heart
beats really fast.
76 8 8 9 7
45.1 43.1 47.2 44.8 45.3 45.2
(SP) I worry what other people think of me. 8 8 7 14 10 6
43.9 39.8 48.2 40.6 44.9 46.0
(PIF) I am scared of insects or spiders. 9 15 4 7 13 10
42.2 28.0 58.6 45.8 41.3 40.7
(SP) I feel afraid to use public toilets or bathrooms. 10 10 10 11 14 8
41.0 38.1 45.1 42.1 39.1 43.6
agoraphobia; this item had also loaded on PA in a sample of young adolescent Australians
(Spence, Barrett, & Turner, 2003). Similarly, the item ‘‘I am scared of the dark,’’ was designed to
contribute to the physical injury fears dimension, but on the SCAS-GR its highest factor loading
was on separation anxiety (the item cross-loaded on PIF). This item also loaded highest on a
separation anxiety disorder factor for a sample of Australian children (Spence, 1998) and cross-
loaded on separation anxiety and fear of physical injury factors in young Australian adolescents
(Spence et al., 2003). These loading patterns are understandable if one considers that darkness
reliably accompanies children’s nocturnal separation from their parents.
More significant discrepancies from the DSM-IV-based theoretical structure of responding on
the SCAS-GR were observed on items designed to measure behaviors specific to generalized
anxiety disorder and to obsessive-compulsive disorder. These findings are relevant to an issue
concerning the content validity of the Spence Children’s Anxiety Scale’s generalized anxiety
disorder subscale as well as to concerns about the content of the DSM-IV diagnostic criteria for
obsessive-compulsive disorder in children.
Chorpita, Yim, Moffitt, Umemoto, and Francis (2000) argued that collectively, the items of the
SCAS GAD subscale were more consistent with the DSM-III-R criteria for overanxious disorder
rather than with the DSM-IV criteria for generalized anxiety disorder, because its items over-
represent muscle tension and autonomic hyperarousal at the expense of excessive worry, the key
characteristic of the DSM-IV conceptualization of GAD (see also Brown, Marten, & Barlow,
1995;Tracey, Chorpita, Douban, & Barlow, 1997). The SCAS-GR factor upon which all six
SCAS generalized anxiety disorder items had their highest factor loading also contained three
items that, while intended for other subscales, are readily interpreted as representing excessive
worries. One of these items, although proposed as a measure of separation anxiety, was a self-
declared worry (‘‘I worry that something awful will happen to my family’’) that had previously
loaded on a generalized anxiety disorder factor in a mixed-ethnicity Hawaiian sample using a
modified SCAS that included seven additional excessive worry items (Chorpita et al., 2000).
The other two items that unexpectedly loaded on the SCAS-GR GAD subscale were intended
to measure obsessions as observed in obsessive-compulsive disorder. The DSM-IV defines
obsessions as ideas, thoughts, impulses or images that are experienced as intrusive, alien or ‘ego-
dystonic’ and are not simply excessive worries. However, both item 19, ‘‘I can’t seem to get bad
or silly thoughts out of my head,’’ and item 41, ‘‘I get bothered by bad or silly thoughts or pictures
in my mind’’ can be interpreted as describing repetitive worries (bad thoughts or images) that
have been denounced by parents or other authorities as excessive, absurd or childish (i.e., silly).
This was apparently the normative interpretation of these items by schoolchildren in Greece, as
self-disclosure on these items was unrelated to responding on the four remaining items of the
SCAS obsessive-compulsive scale. Moreover, items 19 and 41 were the most representative of
the nine SCAS-GR generalized anxiety disorder factor items.
Although these nominal obsessive-compulsive disorder items have not been reported to load
on a GAD factor in previous SCAS studies, on the aforementioned revised SCAS with additional
worry items (Chorpita et al., 2000), item 41 had the lowest factor loading among the items of an
obsessive-compulsive factor, upon which item 19 failed to load. Moreover, two SCAS studies
have reported failures to extract obsessive-compulsive disorder and generalized anxiety disorder
as distinct factors in Japanese, German and South African samples (Essau et al., 2004;Muris,
Schmidt, et al., 2002), although the contribution of responding on the intended obsession items to
these extraction failures is unclear.
These findings might be interpreted as providing support for the view that the original SCAS
generalized anxiety disorder subscale under-represents excessive worry, as children in Hellenic
R.C. Mellon, A.G. Moutavelis / Journal of Anxiety Disorders 21 (2007) 1–21 15
schools would appear, in effect, to have supplemented the GAD scale with three additional
excessive worries. The findings might also be viewed as indicating that the two SCAS obsession
items (at least, in their present Greek-language translation) do not adequately represent this
DSM-IV characteristic of obsessive-compulsive disorder.
Alternatively, however, these findings might be interpreted as indicating that the phenomenon
of obsession itself is poorly differentiated in, or poorly discriminated by, children. Note that one
of the four items of the SCAS-GR compulsive behavior factor describes a non-intrusive
compulsive cognition (item 27, ‘‘I have to think special thoughts to stop bad things from
happening’’); thus, the problem in reporting obsessions does not appear to issue from their
cognitive nature itself. Rather, the problem appears to be specific to the discrimination of their
unbidden or ‘ego-dystonic’ quality.
While repetitive and annoying thoughts are subject to observation, the concept of intrusive
thoughts is based in the assumption that other thoughts are under volitional control, that is to say,
are determined by some sort of personal agency independent of the organism’s history of
interaction with physical events. Such determination is not observable. Of course, people do
experience thoughts as intrusive, but when we measure this phenomenon we are measuring the
frequency with which the subject makes a theoretical abstraction about the causes (or lack
thereof) of his or her vexatious cognition; we do not measure the sources or the mode of
determination of thoughts or images themselves. Thus, in contrast with compulsive cognition,
obsessions are defined by causal attributions; in this sense, obsessions may have more in common
with auditory hallucinations than with compulsive thoughts and ritualistic acts (cf. Garcı
´a-
Montes, Pe
´rez-A
´lvarez, Balbuena, Garcela
´n, & Cangas, in press;Morrison, Haddock, & Tarrier,
1995).
One might well argue that the distinction between voluntary and involuntary thought is
ontologically dubious; at the very least, it would appear to represent a difficult discrimination for
children in Hellenic primary schools, who responded to items intended to represent obsessions in
terms of the worrisome or vexatious content of the thoughts described, rather than their
theoretical provenance in spontaneous mental actuation. The objective difficulty (or
impossibility) of discriminating between reprimanded but repetitive worries and ‘ego-dystonic’
thoughts and images is consistent with the aforementioned difficulties in extracting independent
obsessive-compulsive and general anxiety disorders in previous SCAS studies, as well as with
findings that children report obsessions much less frequently than they report compulsions (e.g.,
Apter et al., 1996;Zohar & Bruno, 1997).
In the present study, a clear four-item compulsive behavior factor was extracted, but as this
factor does not include items describing repetitive thoughts or images experienced as unbidden, it
was named ‘‘compulsive behavior’’ (CB) rather than obsessive-compulsive disorder. The mean
item scores on the CB items were considerably higher than those on other SCAS-GR subscales;
they were also considerably higher than previously reported mean item scores on the six-item
SCAS obsessive-compulsive subscale.
In accord with the aforementioned interpretationofthefactorstructureofrespondingonthe
SCAS-GR, the four items of the compulsive behavior subscale are the only items on the scale
that describe instrumental behaviors that are not worries. Presumably, worries alter
environmental conditions via their effect on other peoples’ behavior, such as when they are
voiced as complaints or as cloaked entreaties for assistance or for the alleviation of demands.
Arguably this social mediation is why, while often effective, worrying is generally not
considered to be mature, manly or desirable. In contrast, the four instrumental behaviors
described in the SCAS-GR compulsive behavior subscale alter environmental conditions
R.C. Mellon, A.G. Moutavelis / Journal of Anxiety Disorders 21 (2007) 1–2116
directly; the repetition of words or phrases, rechecking, reordering and repetitive cleaning, and
the strict maintenance of complex routines all serve to terminate warning signals for disturbing
events (including frightening social events) without the intervention of other people.The
unmediated instrumental nature of the ‘coping’ behaviors described in this subscale might
underlie the finding that, in comparison with scores on other subscales, compulsive behavior
scores were less predictive of total anxiety scores as well as of levels of behaviors specific to
other anxiety disorders.
Because they describe neither the socially mediated worries nor the autonomic responses that
characterize other anxiety disorders (behaviors that are more frequently interpreted as indications
of emotional weakness) it is possible that the relative elevation of scores on the SCAS-GR
compulsive behavior scale is based in part on judgments that the behaviors described in its items
are less undesirable. This interpretation is consistent with a general absence of gender differences
in self-disclosure of compulsive behavior. It has been argued that higher self-reported fear scores
in females are based in normative gender-related differences in the reinforcement and
punishment of fear disclosure, rather than in the levels of fears per se (e.g., Ginsburg &
Silverman, 2000). Higher scores have been reported for female respondents on all SCAS anxiety
disorder subscales except for the obsessive-compulsive subscale, for which gender differences
are smaller or nonexistent (e.g., Spence, 1997, 1998); this finding was replicated in the present
study.
The interpretation that compulsive behavior items (while clearly recognizable as
characteristic of psychopathology by psychologists) were regarded by children in Hellenic
primary schools as more acceptable relative to other anxiety disorder-related behaviors was
supported by the observation that the only subscale for which scores were positively correlated
with a measure of self-regard was the compulsive behavior scale. This positive correlation was
more systematic for boys, for whom self-reporting may be more strongly related, on average, to
the social acceptability of the behaviors disclosed.
Anecdotal evidence is also consistent with the view that the diagnostic characteristics of
obsessive-compulsive disorder might be viewed less pejoratively in the Hellenic Republic
relative to other anxiety disorder-related behaviors. As noted above, academic curricula in our
country are largely based in the rote memorization of material, culminating in highly competitive
entrance examinations determining the quite limited access to tertiary education; examinations
that are likewise based on the accuracy of memorization of extensive texts (Benincasa, 1997).
Memorization is largely a matter of rigid attention, verbal and motor repetition, and checking—
behavioral characteristics that form the core of obsessive-compulsive disorder. In our culture,
academic and other behaviors that do not increase the accuracy of rote memorization are
frequently punished—not just at school, but at home.
Hellenic parents commonly ‘‘diavazoun’ their young children in the after school hours on a
daily basis. In large measure, this means that they test the accuracy of their children’s recall of the
current curriculum material. In effect, this practice provides for yet another check for the
outcome of rigid attention, extensive verbal and motor repetition, and checking. Failures to
perform are often punished (in the least, by a return to the material to be memorized). Such
practices have been regarded as potential determinants of well-structured school performance
fears in Hellenic schoolchildren not observed in other cultures (Mellon et al., 2004); their role in
the determination of behaviors that characterize obsessive-compulsive disorder is now under
investigation.
While Hellenic Republic children’s scores on the compulsive behavior and social phobia
scales were particularly elevated relative to scores from most other countries, a finding also
R.C. Mellon, A.G. Moutavelis / Journal of Anxiety Disorders 21 (2007) 1–21 17
reflected in an analysis of the 10 most frequently endorsed items, our children’s scores were
higher on average on other subscales as well. This is the fourth study of self-reported anxiety
disorder-related behaviors in the Hellenic Republic to find such relative elevations (see Mellon,
2000; Mellon et al., 2004; Simos, 2001).
When interpreting such differences, we must consider the fact that self-report instruments
measure the disclosure of behavior rather than the behaviors disclosed. There is some empirical
evidence that the Hellenic culture encourages emotional self-disclosure more than some other
verbal communities. Specifically, Rosenthal, Efklides, and Demetriou (1988) reported that young
adolescents born and raised in the Hellenic Republic more frequently disclosed problems with
their studies and with their personal relationships in comparison with Australians of Greek and
Anglican ethnicity.
On the other hand, however, there is empirical evidence of strong stigmatization of mental
health difficulties in general in Greece; in a study of adults’ fears, by far the most frequently
endorsed item was to be characterized with psychological problems, with more than 50% of
respondents reporting ‘‘much’’ or ‘‘very much’’ fear of this event (Mellon, 2000). Despite
substantial efforts to raise sensitivity, in the Hellenic Republic persons depicted as having
psychological problems, including anxiety disorders, are still routinely ridiculed in advertising
campaigns, situation comedies, and in everyday dialogue; one might well expect these
circumstances to limit the disclosure of anxiety disorder-related behaviors. Thus, while their
elevated anxiety scores might only be a matter of freer disclosure of emotions, the possibility
remains that relative to most other countries studied to date, people living in the Hellenic
Republic have, on average, more anxiety disorder-related behavior to report.
Beyond the educational and cultural practices already cited, elevated anxiety scores in Greece
might be based in part on more general economic conditions. To date, the only groups to have
produced SCAS scores similar in elevation to those obtained in the Hellenic Republic were from
low, low-middle and middle-socioeconomic classes (but not from a middle-high socioeconomic
class) in a mixed-culture South African sample (Muris, Schmidt, et al., 2002). However, in the
South African study the groups differed in language and ethnicity in addition to socioeconomic
status.
In the present study, children’s anxiety scores were systematically differentiated relative to a
socio-economic indicator (their fathers’ education level) within a single culture; consistent with
the South African data, socio-economically disadvantagedchildreninHellenic primary schools
reported higher levels of anxiety disorder-related behaviors. Moreover, the foreign-born
children of economic immigrants to the Hellenic Republic reported higher levels of anxiety
disorder-related behaviors, on average, in comparison with native-born children. As the
Hellenic Republic is among the poorest countries in Europe, higher average anxiety scores here
relative to those obtained in countries such as Germany and Australia (which are home to many
Greek economic immigrants) might result from the multitude of aversive events associated with
alackofmoneyparticularlyinacountrywithwestern consumer values but comparatively low
incomes.
In the Hellenic Republic, children’s self-reported levels of anxiety disorder-related behaviors
were differentiated by age. While total anxiety scores and scores on five subscales tended to
decrease with increases in age, scores on a social phobia subscale increased with age. These
findings are in general accord with those observed in German and Japanese (Essau et al., 2004)
and Australian (Spence, 1998) schoolchildren with the SCAS, as well as with factor-based fear
scores of Hellenic children on the FSSC-GR, for which only fears of failure and criticism and
school performance fears increased with age (Mellon et al., 2004).
R.C. Mellon, A.G. Moutavelis / Journal of Anxiety Disorders 21 (2007) 1–2118
The generality of this pattern of findings is consistent with the possibility that with the
exception of social phobia, the behaviors that define DSM-IV anxiety disorders do not so much
develop as fail to degenerate during the primary school years; that is to say, older children might
satisfy the diagnostic criteria for these disorders when the emotional and avoidance behaviors
that more commonly characterize younger children have not extinguished or been supplanted by
other age-appropriate behaviors. In contrast, the developmental course of behavior related to
social phobia is consistent with a more general pattern of age-related increases in fear, dread and
avoidance of our conspecifics, cumulating in an adulthood in which anxiety is principally
associated with the presence or the conduct of other human beings.
While its diagnostic utility remains to be directly assessed, the Hellenic Spence Children’s
Anxiety Scale would appear to be a promising research and clinical instrument. Beyond the
aforementioned similarities in the factor structure and psychometric properties of responding on
the SCAS-GR relative to SCAS responding abroad, the intercorrelations among SCAS-GR
subscales were, if different, lower than those previously observed with the SCAS. As the SCAS is
known to discriminate among anxiety disorders in accord with DSM-IV criteria, this finding
provides indirect evidence for the diagnostic utility of the SCAS-GR subscale scores.
Moreover, as would be consistent with a measure of problematic emotional behavior,
teachers’ global assessments of the children’s school performance and adjustment were
negatively correlated with total anxiety scores as well as with scores on each of the six SCAS-GR
anxiety-disorder subscales. These findings also add to a growing body of evidence of an adverse
relation between anxiety disorder-related behavior and intellectual development (Ollendick &
King, 1994;Papay & Speilberger, 1986;Van Ameringen et al., 2003).
Acknowledgments
Many thanks to Dr. Susan H. Spence for her kind permission and encouragement to adapt the
SCAS for Greek language speakers, and to the Hellenic Republic Ministry of Education and the
many children and teachers who participated in this study for their time and effort.
References
Achenbach, T. M. (1991). Manual for the Teacher’s Report Form and 1991-profile. Burlington, VT: University of
Vermont, Department of Psychiatry.
Apter, A., Fallon, T. J., King, R. A., Ratzoni, G., Zohar, A. H., Binder, M., et al. (1996). Obsessive-compulsive
characteristics: from symptoms to syndrome. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry,
35, 907–912.
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington,
DC: APA.
Benincasa, L. (1997). A journey, a struggle, aritual: higher education and the entrance examinations in a Greek province
town (Studies in comparative international education). Stockholm: Stockholm University Institute of International
Education.
Brown, T. A., Marten, P. A., & Barlow, D. H. (1995). Discriminant validity of the symptoms constituting the DSM-III-R
and DSM-IV associated symptom criterion of generalized anxiety disorder. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 9, 317–
328.
Chorpita, B. F., Yim, L., Moffitt, C., Umemoto, L. A., & Francis, S. E. (2000). Assessment of symptoms of DSM-IV
anxiety and depression in children: a revised child anxiety and depression scale. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 38,
835–855.
Essau, C. A., Muris, P., & Ederer, E. M. (2002). Reliability and validity of the ‘‘Spence Children’s Anxiety Scale’’ and the
‘‘Screen for Child Anxiety Related Emotional Disorders’ in German children. Journal of Behavior Therapy and
Experimental Psychiatry, 33, 1–18.
R.C. Mellon, A.G. Moutavelis / Journal of Anxiety Disorders 21 (2007) 1–21 19
Essau, C. A., Sakano, Y., Ishikawa, S., & Sasagawa, S. (2004). Anxiety symptoms in Japanese and in German children.
Behaviour Research and Therapy, 42, 601–612.
Flouris, G., Calogiannakis-Hourdakis, P., Spiridakis, J., & Cambell, J. R. (1994). Tradition and socio-economic status are
Greek keys to academic success. International Journal of Educational Research, 21, 705–711.
Garcı
´a-Montes, J. M., Pe
´rez-A
´lvarez, M., Balbuena, C. S., Garcela
´n, S. P., & Cangas, A. J. (in press). Metacognitions in
patients with hallucinations and obsessive-compulsive disorder: the superstition factor. Behaviour Research and
Therapy.
Ginsburg, G. S., & Silverman, W. K. (2000). Gender role identification and fearfulness in children with anxiety disorders.
Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 14, 57–67.
Ishikawa, S., Oota, R., & Sakano, Y. (2001). Development of the Japanese version of Spence Children’s Anxiety Scale.
Waseda Journal of Clinical Psychology,1, 75 (as cited in Essau, Sakano, Ishikawa, & Sasagawa, 2004).
McCathie, H., & Spence, S. H. (1991). What is the Revised Fear Survey Schedule for Children measuring? Behaviour
Research and Therapy, 29, 495–502.
Mellon, R. (2000). A Greek-language inventory of fears: psychometric properties and factor structure of self-reports of
fears on the Hellenic Fear Survey Schedule. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 22, 123–
140.
Mellon, R., Koliadis, E. A., & Paraskevopoulos, T. D. (2004). Normative development of fears in Greece: self-reports on
the Hellenic Fear Survey Schedule for Children. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 18, 233–254.
Mellon, R., & Moutavelis, A. (2005). Parental practices and school performance of public school students. School and
Family: Panhellenic Conference of the Psychological Society of Northern Greece, Ioannina, Greece, March.
Morrison, A. P., Haddock, G., & Tarrier, N. (1995). Intrusive thoughts and auditory hallucinations: a cognitive approach.
Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 23, 265–280.
Muris, P., Merckelbach, H., Ollendick, T., King, N., & Bogie, N. (2002). Three traditional and three new childhood
anxiety questionnaires: their reliability and validity in a normal adolescent sample. Behaviour Research and Therapy,
40, 753–772.
Muris, P., Schmidt, H., Engelbrecht, P., & Perold, M. (2002). DSM-IV-defined anxiety disorder symptoms in South
African children. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 41, 1360–1368.
Muris, P., Schmidt, H., & Merckelbach, H. (2000). Correlations among two self-report questionnaires for measuring
DSM-defined anxiety disorder symptoms in children: the Screen for Child Anxiety Related Emotional Disorders and
the Spence Children’s Anxiety Scale. Personality and Individual Differences, 28, 333–346.
Ollendick, T. H. (1983). Reliability and validity of the Revised Fear Survey Schedule for Children (FSSC-R). Behaviour
Research and Therapy, 21, 685–692.
Ollendick, T. H., & King, N. J. (1994). Fears and their levels of interference in adolescents. Behaviour Research and
Therapy, 32, 635–638.
Papay, J. P., & Speilberger, C. D. (1986). Assessment of anxiety and achievement in kindergarten and first- and second-
grade children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 14, 279–286.
Perrin, S., & Last, C. G. (1992). Do childhood anxiety measures measure anxiety? Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology,
20, 567–577.
Rosenthal, D. A., Efklides, A., & Demetriou, A. (1988). Parental criticism and young adolescent self-disclosure: a cross-
cultural study. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 17, 25–39.
Simos, G. (2001). Childhood fears: developmental and transcultural perspectives. Thessaloniki: University Studio Press.
(in Greek).
Spence, S. H. (1997). Structure of anxiety symptoms among children: a confirmatory factor-analytic study. Journal of
Abnormal Psychology, 106, 280–297.
Spence, S. H. (1998). A measure of anxiety symptoms among children. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 36, 545–
566.
Spence, S. H., Barrett, P. M., & Turner, C. M. (2003). Psychometric properties of the Spence Children’s Anxiety Scale
with young adolescents. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 17, 605–625.
Stallings, P., & March, J. S. (1995). Assessment. In: J. S. March (Ed.), Anxiety disorders in children and adolescents (pp.
125–147). New York: Guilford Press.
Tracey, S. A., Chorpita, B. F., Douban, J., & Barlow, D. H. (1997). Empirical evaluation of DSM-IV generalized anxiety
disorder and adolescents. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 26, 404–414.
Van Ameringen, M., Mancini, C., & Farvolden, P. (2003). The impact of anxiety disorders on educational achievement.
Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 17, 561–571.
van de Vijver, F., & Hambleton, R. K. (1996). Translating tests: some practical guidelines. European Psychologist, 1, 89–
99.
R.C. Mellon, A.G. Moutavelis / Journal of Anxiety Disorders 21 (2007) 1–2120
Weems, C. F., Silverman, W. K., Saavedra, L. M., Pina, A. A., & White Lumpkin, P. (1999). Discrimination of children’s
phobias using the Revised Fear Survey Schedule for Children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 40, 941–
952.
Wolpe, J., & Lang, P. J. (1977). Manual for the Fear Survey Schedule. San Diego, CA: Educational and Industrial Testing
Service.
Zohar, A. H., & Bruno, R. (1997). Normative and pathological obsessive-compulsive behavior and ideation in childhood:
a question of timing. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38, 993–999.
R.C. Mellon, A.G. Moutavelis / Journal of Anxiety Disorders 21 (2007) 1–21 21
... Also, according to Weems and Costa, (2005) children with SLD tend to be more vulnerable to failure and social criticism, showing symptoms of Social Disorder Anxiety, called otherwise as Social Phobia, while Carol and Iles, (2006) in their research found that SLD was more associated with Separation Anxiety. Respectively, the research of Mellon and Moutavelis (2007) with a sample of Greek students showed that the levels of stress that coexisted with SLD were negatively correlated even with school adjustment and performance, decreased though over time, with the sole exception of Social phobia, which followed an upward course. The research of Bozas, Bonti, Kouimtzi, Kyritsis and Karageorgiou (2016) was in the same line of results: it was found that students with SLD showed higher rates of antisocial behaviors or stressful outbursts, especially towards their peers, thus influencing key areas of their daily lives and their social acceptance and integration. ...
... On all subscales of CATS, SCAS-CH, and SCAS-P questionnaires, gender did not appear to play a significant role in the sample members' responses. This result contradicts the findings of similar studies, which emphasize the tendency of girls to have more frequent and more severe anxiety symptoms in childhood (Spence, 1998;Mellon & Moutavelis, 2007). ...
... There are no relevant studies to explain this kind of symptoms in the whole student population. However, it should be noted that Mellon and Moutavelis (2007), using the same tool to detect anxiety problems in Greek students, found equally increased rates on this scale compared to other nationalities. This finding was attributed to cultural and religious peculiarities according to which Obsessive / Compulsive Disorder is considered more acceptable in Greek culture compared to other psychiatric disorders. ...
Article
Full-text available
p>Special Learning Disabilities (SLD) are often accompanied by negative thoughts of self-assessment, deficits in social interactions and psycho-emotional problems as well, which hinder the development and the smooth integration of children diagnosed with SLD into their environment. This research attempts to investigate the association of negative self-assessment thoughts and anxiety disorders with Special Learning Disabilities in a sample of sixty Greek-speaking children in 4th, 5th and 6th grades with and without SLD, including their parents. The tools used for this purpose are the Children’s Automatic Thoughts Scale (CATS), and the Spence Children’s Anxiety Scale (SCAS-CH) self-report questionnaire, which assess anxiety disorders. Parents were asked to answer the corresponding Spence Children’s Anxiety Scale-Parents Edition (SCAS-P) symptom-monitoring questionnaire. According to the results compared to their normal development peers, children with SLD had a higher incidence of negative thoughts, which is more likely to lead to symptoms of anxiety disorders; their parents in fact were able to perceive the severity or frequency of these symptoms in their children. In addition, the frequency of negative thoughts and symptoms related to anxiety disorders seemed to be influenced by the student’s condition (with or without SLD), but not by gender and age. Article visualizations: </p
... Data sources were the following: (1) the Greek translation of the psychometric tool Spence Children's Anxiety Scale (SCAS) to measure children's anxiety symptoms [43][44][45], (2) 3 open-ended questions to assess the ability to recognize emotions and anxiety symptoms and to recall ways of managing stress, and (3) the System Usability Scale (SUS) to measure the usability of the learning environment. These 3 data sources were used to answer research questions 1, 2, and 3, respectively. ...
... These 6 statements are not typically included in the data analysis. The scale was translated and weighted for the Greek population [45]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background Resilience is a person’s mental ability to deal with challenging situations adaptively and is a crucial stress management skill. Psychological resilience and finding ways to cope in crises is a highly relevant topic considering the COVID-19 pandemic, which enforced quarantine, social distancing measures, and school closures worldwide. Parents and children are currently living with increased stress due to COVID-19. We need to respond with immediate ways to strengthen children’s resilience. Internet-based cognitive behavioral therapy interventions for children's stress management overcome accessibility issues such as the inability to visit mental health experts owing to COVID-19 movement restrictions. An interactive learning environment was created, based on the preventive program “Friends,” to overcome accessibility issues associated with delivering cognitive behavioral therapy–based interventions in formal and informal education settings. Objective This study aimed to examine the effectiveness of a web-based learning environment on resilience in (1) reducing anxiety symptoms and (2) increasing emotion recognition and recognition of stress management techniques among 9-10-year-old children. We also aimed to evaluate the learning environment’s usability. Methods A quasi-experimental pretest-posttest control group design was used. In total, 20 fourth graders in the experimental group interacted with the learning environment over 6 weekly 80-minute sessions. Further, 21 fourth graders constituted the control group. The main data sources were (1) a psychometric tool to measure children’s anxiety symptoms, namely the Greek translation of the original Spence Children’s Anxiety Scale, (2) 3 open-ended questions assessing emotion recognition and recognition of stress management techniques, and (3) the System Usability Scale to measure the usability of the learning environment. Results In both groups, there was a small but nonsignificant postintervention reduction in reported anxiety symptoms, except for obsessive-compulsive disorder symptoms in the experimental group. A paired samples t test revealed that students’ reported symptom scores of obsessive-compulsive disorder significantly decreased from 1.06 (SD 0.68) to 0.76 (SD 0.61) (t19= 5.16; P=.01). The experimental group revealed a significant increase in emotion recognition (t19=–6.99; P<.001), identification of somatic symptoms of stress (t19=–7.31; P<.001), and identification of stress management techniques (t19=–6.85; P<.001). The learning environment received a satisfactory usability score. The raw average system usability score was 76.75 (SD 8.28), which is in the 80th percentile rank and corresponds to grade B. Conclusions This study shows that interactive learning environments might deliver resilience interventions in an accessible and cost-effective manner in formal education, potentially even in distance-learning conditions owing to the COVID-19 pandemic. Interactive learning environments on resilience are also valuable tools for parents who can use them with their children at home, for informal learning, using mobile devices. As such, they could be a promising first-step, low-intensity intervention that children and the youth can easily access.
... The measure was previously translated into Greek, and an additional item, "Fear of elevators", was added [32]. The Greek translation has shown good psychometric properties [32]. ...
... The measure was previously translated into Greek, and an additional item, "Fear of elevators", was added [32]. The Greek translation has shown good psychometric properties [32]. Internal consistency for the total (39-item) scale in the current sample was α = 0.90. ...
Article
Full-text available
High anxiety and anxiety disorders are among the most prevalent mental health problems in children and lead to significant interference with children’s daily functioning. Most empirical evaluations of treatment come from English-language countries. The aim of the present study was to evaluate and replicate the effectiveness of a cognitive-behavioral intervention program to manage anxiety in children among children from Greece. Forty-one children–parent(s) dyads participated in the study. Children were 9–12 years old, with clinically elevated symptoms of anxiety, and they were assigned to either the standard group treatment (cognitive behavior therapy (CBT)) or to a waitlist group (WL). Both children and their parents in the CBT group reported statistically significant reductions in children’s anxiety symptoms at post-intervention and at the 6-month follow-up. A significant reduction was also found in life interference due to anxiety according to both child and parent reports. In contrast, no significant changes in anxiety symptoms or life interference were reported among the WL. The current results support the effectiveness of a CBT program for anxious children from a non-clinic, non-school setting in Greece.
Article
Full-text available
The Spence Children’s Anxiety Scale (SCAS) is one of the most used instruments to assess anxiety symptoms in children and adolescents. Extensive research has been conducted to examine its psychometric properties and to develop other versions of the scale. The objective of this study was to examine the psychometric properties and factorial structure of the SCAS across different versions and populations. This systematic review followed PRISMA guidelines and was registered. APA PsycINFO, Web of Science (Core Collection) and MEDLINE (PubMed) were searched. Fifty-two studies were included in this systematic review. They examined the factor structure, convergent and divergent validity, and internal consistency of the scale. The most supported model was the original six-factor model, followed by the higher order six-factor model for the long version of the SCAS. Studies provided evidence of convergent validity and internal consistency. It is concluded that the SCAS is a valid and reliable instrument for assessing anxiety symptoms in children and adolescents, with a six-factor model structure well supported in most populations. Further research on the psychometric properties and factor structure of other versions of the scale and its application to clinical populations is warranted.
Article
Full-text available
Evidence-based information is essential for effective mental health care, yet the extent and accessibility of the scientific literature are critical barriers for professionals and policymakers. To map the necessities and make validated resources accessible, we undertook a systematic review of scientific evidence on child and adolescent mental health in Greece encompassing three research topics: prevalence estimates, assessment instruments, and interventions. We searched Pubmed, Web of Science, PsycINFO, Google Scholar, and IATPOTEK from inception to December 16th, 2021. We included studies assessing the prevalence of conditions, reporting data on assessment tools, and experimental interventions. For each area, manuals informed data extraction and the methodological quality were ascertained using validated tools. This review was registered in protocols.io [68583]. We included 104 studies reporting 533 prevalence estimates, 223 studies informing data on 261 assessment instruments, and 34 intervention studies. We report the prevalence of conditions according to regions within the country. A repository of locally validated instruments and their psychometrics was compiled. An overview of interventions provided data on their effectiveness. The outcomes are made available in an interactive resource online [https://rpubs.com/camhi/sysrev_table]. Scientific evidence on child and adolescent mental health in Greece has now been cataloged and appraised. This timely and accessible compendium of up-to-date evidence offers valuable resources for clinical practice and policymaking in Greece and may encourage similar assessments in other countries.
Article
Background Previous studies have shown that the prevalence of anxiety among primary school students in rural areas is relatively high. Also, this stage is a critical developmental stage of academic life. Childhood anxiety disorders lead to significant disruption and interference with other aspects of life, including behavioral, emotional, cognitive, and academic achievement. Aims This study aimed to find the frequency of 6 subtypes of anxiety disorders and to determine the potential sociodemographic factors of anxiety disorders among primary school students in a rural area of Malaysia. Methods This was a cross-sectional study in which 411 students answered the Spence Children’s Anxiety Scale-Child version (SCAS-C). Descriptive analyses elucidated at-risk students, and multivariate analysis of variance and multiple linear regression presented potential predictors of anxiety disorders. Results The most common abnormal level was for social phobia (SP) (n = 109/18.6%), and the least common were panic/agoraphobia (n = 85/14.1%) and physical injury fears (PIF) (n = 82/13.6%). Multivariate analysis of variance revealed that age ( P < .05), gender ( P < .05), father’s employment ( P < .05), and mother’s educational level (EL) P < .05) were predictors of overall anxiety. A Tukey post-hoc test revealed that older children are more likely to suffer from overall anxiety, separation anxiety (SA), SP, and obsessive-compulsive problems. Children of low-income families were less likely to suffer from SA than those of middle-income families. Children of stay-at-home mothers were less likely to suffer from overall anxiety, SA, and SP than mothers who worked more than 8 h per day. Multiple linear regression could predict 6 models of anxiety based on sociodemographic factors. Conclusion According to our findings in this study, promoting mental health by providing preventive strategies and screening programs is more recommended for students with sociodemographic risk factors for anxiety disorders.
Article
The Spence Children's Anxiety Scale (SCAS) is a tool for measuring anxiety symptoms in children and adolescents. In this study, the psychometric properties of the Persian version of SCAS were investigated in an Iranian adolescents. This study was conducted on a sample of 684 adolescents. For standardization of SCAS, first- and second-order confirmatory factor analyses wereperformed. Also, to evaluate convergent and divergent validity, Fornell and Larcker criteria (1994), along with the Revised Children's Manifest Anxiety Scale (RCMAS)and Children's Depression Inventory (CDI), was used. A receiver operating characteristic (ROC) curve analysis was also performedto determine the cut-off point. The model fit of the correlated six-factor model was good however, a second-order model provided a statistically superior fit to the data. The reliability coefficients for the total scale and its dimensions were satisfactory (α > 0.7). Therefore, it can be concluded that the Persian version of SCAS has acceptable reliability and validity and can be used as a useful tool for early screening of anxiety in Iranian adolescents due to its easy use and specific design for children and adolescents.
Article
Full-text available
Background: The COVID-19 crisis influenced the lives of families and preschoolers, worldwide. School closures and restriction measures introduced distance learning for preschoolers and remote working for parents. Social distancing narrowed opportunities to meet with peers and enjoy leisure activities. Additionally, social and mental services closures limited young children's accessibility to mental, speech and occupational health services. The aim of the current study was to investigate how home confinement during the third wave of the COVID-19 pandemic affected parenting self-efficacy and preschoolers' anxiety. Method: An online survey based on a convenience sample took place on April 2021 to evaluate how home confinement to halt the third wave of COVID-19 pandemic influenced children's anxiety and parenting self-efficacy (PSE). Parents of 146 children (65 girls [44.5%] and 81 boys [55.5%]; aged 2-6 years old) were enrolled and completed a demographics form, the Preschool Anxiety Scale (PAS) and the Tool to Measure Parenting Self-efficacy (TOPSE). Results: Most of the participants reported that the relationship with their child was positively affected from staying at home. TOPSE mean scores reflected average parenting self-efficacy. PSE was negatively correlated with children's anxiety. COVID-19-related variables 'Parent's vaccine hesitancy' and 'Death of a loved one' had a clear effect on preschoolers' anxiety, whereas the latter also on PSE. Conclusion: Findings highlight the necessity of implementing public health strategies to strengthen families and support parents and their children during the ongoing health crisis. Keywords: children's stress; COVID-19 lockdown; parenting self-efficacy; preschoolers' anxiety; PAS scale; TOPSE scale.
Article
The purpose of this study was to examine the effects of an 8-week stress management intervention to enhance resilience and coping techniques and decrease stress in adolescent students. Teenagers, 11 to 17 years old, recruited from two tertiary Adolescent Medicine Centers of the National and Kapodistrian University of Athens, Greece, were randomly assigned into two groups: the stress management group (n=24) and the control group (n=25). Resilience, stress, anxiety, everyday use of social media, school performance and cognitive skills were measured in adolescents of both groups, pre- and post-intervention. Post-intervention, the stress management group had significantly higher resilience scores and school performance self-evaluation scores, lower scores of stress, anxiety and everyday use of social media and better cognitive skills than the control group. Regarding cognitive skills, the stress management group significantly improved the speed of information processing and memory. Adolescents following stress management experienced significantly reduced stress from interacting with teachers/parents, from peer pressure, from school/leisure conflict as well as compulsive behaviours. With respect to resilience, the intervention improved adolescents’ individual skills and resources, relationships with primary caregivers, and environmental factors that facilitated the sense of belonging. Future studies of large adolescent samples are required to evaluate the long-term benefits of stress management techniques on adolescents' health and resilience, as well as the need of continued support to preserve these benefits throughout transition to adulthood.
Article
Full-text available
Discusses the translation of psychological instruments in cross-cultural research. A taxonomy of bias ranging from unobserved ethnocentrism in constructs to incorrect word choice in translations is provided. Three types of bias are distinguished: (1) construct bias (related to nonequivalence of constructs across cultural groups), (2) method bias (resulting from instrument administration problems), and (3) item bias (often a result of inadequate translations such as incorrect word choice). Ways in which bias can affect the adequacy of instruments are illustrated. Guidelines for test translations are outlined and are fully described. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Full-text available
This study was designed to test whether obsessive-compulsive behavior declines over development. A cross-sectional design was used on a large community sample of children. Children in grades four, six, and eight (N= 1083), 8 to 14 years of age, were administered the Maudsley Obsessive Compulsive Inventory (MOCI) and the Spielberger State Trait Anxiety Scales. Whereas the mean MOCI score was significantly lower in the eighth grade than in the sixth and fourth, there was an elevation of children with very high MOCI scores in the eighth grade. Obsessive ideas and compulsive behaviors that were common for fourth-grade children were present in only a minority of children in the eighth grade, and were associated with high levels of anxiety. No gender differences were observed for overall obsessive-compulsive behavior, but checking behavior was higher in boys, and cleaning behavior in girls. State anxiety was higher in girls than in boys, and was also higher in older than in younger children.
Article
This study examined the degree to which anxiety symptoms among children cluster into subtypes of anxiety problems consistent with Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (4th edition) classification of anxiety disorders. Two community samples of 698 children 8-12 years of age completed a questionnaire regarding the frequency with which they experienced a wide range of anxiety symptoms. Confirmatory factor analysis of responses from Cohort 1 indicated that a model involving 6 discrete but correlated factors, reflecting the areas of panic-agoraphobia, social phobia, separation anxiety, obsessive-compulsive problems, generalized anxiety, and physical fears, provided an excellent fit of the data. The high level of covariance between latent factors was satisfactorily explained by a higher order model in which each 1st-order factor loaded on a single 2nd-order factor. The findings were replicated with Cohort 2 and were equivalent across genders.
Article
Criticism by mothers and fathers, as well as young adolescents' perceptions of parental criticism and their self-disclosure to parents, was assessed for a sample of 80 families. Of these, 40 were resident in Australia (20 Anglo-Australian and 20 Greek-Australian) and 40 were resident in Greece (20 professional and 20 working-class). There were no differences between the groups in amount of criticism by parents nor in adolescents' perceptions of criticism. Greek- and Anglo-Australian adolescents disclosed significantly less to parents than did the Greek adolescents. For Greek-Australian adolescents there was an inverse relationship between self-disclosure on a number of topics and perceived levels of parental criticism. The results were interpreted in terms of cultural differences between the groups and adaptive behaviors of the Greek-Australian adolescents.
Article
This chapter describes two studies of parental influence conducted in Greece. The first is a national study that employed a stratified random sample of 759, 5th- and 6th-grade students throughout Greece; the second is a qualitative study of 20 families of high achieving students. Both studies examine the effects of SES variables, family processes, and academic and general self-concepts on children's language and math achievement. The results of these studies show that extensive differential socialization exists in Greece, especially in rural communities. Urban children in higher SES families were found to be more successful achievers. The more successful parents used low pressure, less help and more flexibility in supervising school work. A series of interesting propositions concludes the chapter.
Article
This study assessed the psychometric properties of self-reports on the Hellenic Fear Survey Schedule for Children (FSSC-GR), a Greek-language version of Ollendick’s FSSC-R [Behav. Res. Ther. 21 (1983) 685–692], and explored the factor structure, intensity, prevalence, and content of fears of Hellenic children aged 7–12 years. Internal consistency and temporal stability assessments of responding on the FSSC-GR were comparable to those obtained on the FSSC-R. A seven-factor solution provided the best conceptual fit for the structure of children’s fears in Greece, including five components similar to factors previously obtained for the FSSC-R, plus two unique components, “Travel and Agoraphobic Fears” and “School Performance Fears.” Fear intensity and prevalence scores of Hellenic children were higher than scores observed in most countries. Self-reported fear scores were higher for Hellenic girls than for boys. The relation between age and self-reported fears in Greece was complex and interpreted in relation to age-related changes in environmental demands.
Article
In this paper, an account of auditory hallucinations is outlined, incorporating the phenomena of intrusive thoughts; this is done with reference to the prevailing cognitive models of auditory hallucinations. The account proposes that metacognitive beliefs inconsistent with intrusive thoughts lead to their external attribution as auditory hallucinations, and that such a misattribution is maintained by reducing cognitive dissonance. It is also suggested that the appraisal of the resulting hallucinatory experience elicits behavioural, emotional and physiological responses that may be involved in the maintenance process. The possibility of extending such an account to certain other positive symptoms is discussed, and the theoretical and clinical implications of such an account are considered, and illustrated with a case example. Finally, a number of testable predictions are made.