a , 3D best direction of each unit versus their locations in deltoid, for each subject. The best direction is given by the direction of the unit vector from the origin. The color of the vector codes for location. As location changes from posterior to anterior (and color changes from the blue to the red end of the spectrum), best direction changes from down – out–backward to up–out–forward. b , Pooled 3D best directions of all units versus their normalized locations in deltoid, for all subjects. Best directions are coded for location as in a . 

a , 3D best direction of each unit versus their locations in deltoid, for each subject. The best direction is given by the direction of the unit vector from the origin. The color of the vector codes for location. As location changes from posterior to anterior (and color changes from the blue to the red end of the spectrum), best direction changes from down – out–backward to up–out–forward. b , Pooled 3D best directions of all units versus their normalized locations in deltoid, for all subjects. Best directions are coded for location as in a . 

Source publication
Article
Full-text available
The directional activity of whole muscles has been shown to be broadly and often multimodally tuned, raising the question of how this tuning is subserved at the level of single motor units (SMUs). Previously defined rules of SMU activation would predict that units of the same muscle (or at least of the same neuromuscular compartment) are activated...

Contexts in source publication

Context 1
... the down–backward direction. In the horizontal and frontal planes, the threshold line yielded a best direction of out–back- ward and out–downward, respectively. The orientation of the threshold plane in the bottom row illustrates that the 3D best direction of unit 10.1 was in the out–back–downward direction. Unit 10.2 was found in a recording location 6.5 cm anterior to that of unit 10.1 (i.e., in the medial deltoid). Its best direction in the sagittal plane was in the up–forward direction, comparable to that of unit 9.2. Considering only the sagittal plane, one might thus conclude that the best directions for units 10.1 and 10.2 differed by 180°. The threshold lines for the horizontal and frontal planes, however, show that the best directions for unit 10.2 in these planes differed from those of unit 10.1 by only 60 and 15°, respectively. With respect to the lines for unit 10.1, the threshold lines for unit 10.2 were slightly rotated counterclockwise, such that the best direction was out–forward for the horizontal plane and straight outward for the frontal plane. Accordingly, the plane fit to the 3D data yielded a best direction for unit 10.2 that was oriented out–forward and slightly up. Of the three units in Figure 10, unit 10.3 was found in the most anterior recording location. Considering only the sagittal plane, this unit appeared to have approximately the same best direction as unit 10.2; however, the threshold lines for the other two planes were rotated counterclockwise with respect to those of unit 10.2 (by 30 and 35° for the horizontal and frontal planes, respectively). The best directions for these planes are thus forward– out (with a greater forward component than that of unit 10.2) in the hori- zontal plane and out–up in the frontal plane. The orientation of the threshold plane in 3D confirms that the best direction of this unit is up – out–forward. It follows from these results that the very large (or very small) differences found between the best directions of deltoid units for forces in the sagittal plane are deceptive. Units with apparently opposite best directions in the sagittal plane (such as units 10.1 and 10.2) might in fact have threshold planes that differ only relatively little in their orientations. Thus, in 3D space the dif- ference between the best directions of units 10.1 and 10.2 was only 53°. On the other hand, units with approximately the same best direction in the sagittal plane might show significant differ- ences between their best directions in 3D. Thus, the difference between the 3D orientation of the planes of units 10.2 and 10.3 was 48°, although their threshold lines in the sagittal plane dif- fered by only 6°. Analogous to the bimodal units described above for biceps (e.g., unit 6.3) we also found evidence for multiple directional inputs on deltoid SMUs (19%, n ϭ 14 in the 2D experiment (Fig. 9), 7%, n ϭ 5 in the 3D experiments). The 3D recruitment data for a bidirectional deltoid unit are shown in Figure 11. From the sagittal plane data, which are best fit by two approximately par- allel threshold lines, one cannot discern whether the threshold planes in 3D would be parallel, yielding two best directions opposite to each other or whether the two planes would have a line of intersection. However, two parallel threshold planes would intersect all three experimental planes with two parallel lines, and the two-line fits to the horizontal and frontal plane data clearly show that this is not the case. The two lines in these plots are not parallel but instead intersect at an angle of 90° in the horizontal and 125° in the frontal plane. Accordingly, the two threshold planes fit to the 3D data have a line of intersection. The best directions for this unit are up – out–forward and down – out–back- ward and differ by 93° in 3D. None of the threshold data of bidirectional units found in the second set of experiments were fit by two parallel planes, meaning that all bidirectional units exhib- ited intersecting threshold planes. The differences between the two best directions of such units ranged from 58 to 96° in 3D space with an average value of 77°. In Figure 12, each deltoid SMU is represented by a unit vector radiating (from the origin) in the best direction of the unit. The color of the vector codes for the anterior–posterior location of the unit in the muscle. In general, as unit location changed from posterior (blue end of spectrum) to anterior (red end), best direction gradually changed from down –backward to up– forward. A similar pattern was found in all three subjects (Fig. 12 a ). Regressing recording location on the x , y , z coordinate of the tip of the unit vector yielded significant correlations for subject A ( p Ͻ 0.001, r ϭ 0.79) and subject B ( p Ͻ 0.05, r ϭ 0.71) but a nonsignificant correlation for subject D ( p ϭ 0.066, r ϭ 0.66). Because all subjects exhibited a qualitatively similar relation between best direction and location, we then pooled the data (Fig. 12 b ). Pooling data from all subjects by normalizing the unit location (across the width of each subject’s deltoid, see Fig. 13 b ) yielded a highly significant relation between the 3D best direction and location ( p Ͻ 0.001, r ϭ 0.64). When regressing the 2D best directions for the horizontal and frontal planes on unit location, highly significant correlations ( p Ͻ 0.001) were obtained for all three subjects in both planes, with r 2 -values ranging from 0.6 to 0.8 (plots not shown). Units located in the posterior part of the muscle were best activated for out–backward (horizontal plane) and out– downward (frontal plane) forces. As unit location changed to the more anterior part of the muscle, best directions gradually changed to the out–forward and out–upward directions, with no suggestion of discontinuity at posterior–medial or medi- al–anterior compartmental boundaries. The range of threshold magnitudes in our sample of BI and deltoid units extended from 0.2 to 28 N, and ϳ 50% of the units had threshold forces of Ͼ 12 N. Thus, a substantial number of units in both muscles were recruited at relatively high force levels and could be assumed to contract quickly enough to be involved in the generation of fast-rising forces or movements. Moreover, as will be explained below, we found that units with threshold magnitudes as low as 2 N were involved in the generation of fast pulses of isometric force. Figure 13 illustrates the results from an experiment in which subject D produced isometric pulses to different targets in the frontal plane. (This figure is also representative of the results of the second dynamic experiment.) Simultaneous recordings from two different electrodes are shown. The threshold lines and preferred directions (in the frontal plane) of two units found on the two different electrodes are shown in Figure 13 a . The best direction of unit MD (found in the center of deltoid, Fig. 13 b ) was almost straight outward with only a slight upward component. On the other hand, unit M/AD (found in a recording location ϳ 3 cm anterior to unit MD) had an up–forward best direction. Consider now the relative timing of the multiunit bursts on the two recording electrodes during dynamic pulses (with a 250 msec time to peak) in the three different directions (Fig. 13 c , d ). As the direction of the pulse changed from upward ( top lines ) to up–out ( bottom lines ), the bursts in the two recordings reversed their order. For the upward direction (closer to best direction of unit M/AD), the burst in the recording at the more anterior site was earlier. For the outward direction (closer to best direction of unit MD), the burst in the recording at the more posterior site was earlier. Because we have ascertained that units M/AD and MD are involved in those bursts (as marked in the three-trial unit rasters), it appears that the recruitment order of these individual units also changed depending on the direction of the dynamic force. We found that threshold data for 93% of our biceps and deltoid motor units could be fit with lines in 2D and /or planes in 3D, consistent with a model in which activation levels are tuned as a cosine f unction of force direction (Fig. 2) and in consonance with the well established idea that the descending inputs to spinal motoneuronal –interneuronal pools may have cosine-tuned activ- ity (Georgopoulos et al., 1982, 1988; Fortier et al., 1993). Using the orientation of the line or plane as a measure of the best direction of a unit, we showed that SMUs of the same muscle were not activated homogeneously but instead different units had different best directions. The best directions of units changed continuously with their locations in the muscle and did not cluster into distinct groups. For deltoid, the gradual change in best directions may echo the gradual change in the mechanical actions of the muscle fibers (Buneo et al., 1996). As discussed below, the gradual change of best directions in biceps might be understood by considering that during the time course of a movement, biceps units may act in synergy with various deltoid units. Thus, the directional specificity of the recruitment order may act in con- junction with the phenomenon of size-ordered recruitment, to enable smooth and fatigue-resistant muscle contractions. But before developing these ideas, we will consider several technical issues that influence the interpretation of our data. Using only a 2 df force transducer in the first set of experiments raised the concern that any significant differences between 2D best directions of different BI units might be caused by day-to-day variability in the levels of force outside the sagittal plane. This possibility was ruled out by the fact that unit recordings obtained under identical force conditions (on the same electrode and/or in the same recording session) yielded significantly different best directions (Fig. 5, Table 1). Moreover, we were often able to identify two ...
Context 2
... Ͻ 0.01 level. The values of the correlation coefficient r for the x , y , and z coordinates of the threshold force levels ranged from 0.40 to 0.95. These results illustrate that in consonance with the line fit to the 2D data, threshold data in 3D can be fit by a plane. In Figure 10, the threshold line for unit 10.1 in the sagittal plane was oriented much like that for unit 9.1, with a best direction in the down–backward direction. In the horizontal and frontal planes, the threshold line yielded a best direction of out–back- ward and out–downward, respectively. The orientation of the threshold plane in the bottom row illustrates that the 3D best direction of unit 10.1 was in the out–back–downward direction. Unit 10.2 was found in a recording location 6.5 cm anterior to that of unit 10.1 (i.e., in the medial deltoid). Its best direction in the sagittal plane was in the up–forward direction, comparable to that of unit 9.2. Considering only the sagittal plane, one might thus conclude that the best directions for units 10.1 and 10.2 differed by 180°. The threshold lines for the horizontal and frontal planes, however, show that the best directions for unit 10.2 in these planes differed from those of unit 10.1 by only 60 and 15°, respectively. With respect to the lines for unit 10.1, the threshold lines for unit 10.2 were slightly rotated counterclockwise, such that the best direction was out–forward for the horizontal plane and straight outward for the frontal plane. Accordingly, the plane fit to the 3D data yielded a best direction for unit 10.2 that was oriented out–forward and slightly up. Of the three units in Figure 10, unit 10.3 was found in the most anterior recording location. Considering only the sagittal plane, this unit appeared to have approximately the same best direction as unit 10.2; however, the threshold lines for the other two planes were rotated counterclockwise with respect to those of unit 10.2 (by 30 and 35° for the horizontal and frontal planes, respectively). The best directions for these planes are thus forward– out (with a greater forward component than that of unit 10.2) in the hori- zontal plane and out–up in the frontal plane. The orientation of the threshold plane in 3D confirms that the best direction of this unit is up – out–forward. It follows from these results that the very large (or very small) differences found between the best directions of deltoid units for forces in the sagittal plane are deceptive. Units with apparently opposite best directions in the sagittal plane (such as units 10.1 and 10.2) might in fact have threshold planes that differ only relatively little in their orientations. Thus, in 3D space the dif- ference between the best directions of units 10.1 and 10.2 was only 53°. On the other hand, units with approximately the same best direction in the sagittal plane might show significant differ- ences between their best directions in 3D. Thus, the difference between the 3D orientation of the planes of units 10.2 and 10.3 was 48°, although their threshold lines in the sagittal plane dif- fered by only 6°. Analogous to the bimodal units described above for biceps (e.g., unit 6.3) we also found evidence for multiple directional inputs on deltoid SMUs (19%, n ϭ 14 in the 2D experiment (Fig. 9), 7%, n ϭ 5 in the 3D experiments). The 3D recruitment data for a bidirectional deltoid unit are shown in Figure 11. From the sagittal plane data, which are best fit by two approximately par- allel threshold lines, one cannot discern whether the threshold planes in 3D would be parallel, yielding two best directions opposite to each other or whether the two planes would have a line of intersection. However, two parallel threshold planes would intersect all three experimental planes with two parallel lines, and the two-line fits to the horizontal and frontal plane data clearly show that this is not the case. The two lines in these plots are not parallel but instead intersect at an angle of 90° in the horizontal and 125° in the frontal plane. Accordingly, the two threshold planes fit to the 3D data have a line of intersection. The best directions for this unit are up – out–forward and down – out–back- ward and differ by 93° in 3D. None of the threshold data of bidirectional units found in the second set of experiments were fit by two parallel planes, meaning that all bidirectional units exhib- ited intersecting threshold planes. The differences between the two best directions of such units ranged from 58 to 96° in 3D space with an average value of 77°. In Figure 12, each deltoid SMU is represented by a unit vector radiating (from the origin) in the best direction of the unit. The color of the vector codes for the anterior–posterior location of the unit in the muscle. In general, as unit location changed from posterior (blue end of spectrum) to anterior (red end), best direction gradually changed from down –backward to up– forward. A similar pattern was found in all three subjects (Fig. 12 a ). Regressing recording location on the x , y , z coordinate of the tip of the unit vector yielded significant correlations for subject A ( p Ͻ 0.001, r ϭ 0.79) and subject B ( p Ͻ 0.05, r ϭ 0.71) but a nonsignificant correlation for subject D ( p ϭ 0.066, r ϭ 0.66). Because all subjects exhibited a qualitatively similar relation between best direction and location, we then pooled the data (Fig. 12 b ). Pooling data from all subjects by normalizing the unit location (across the width of each subject’s deltoid, see Fig. 13 b ) yielded a highly significant relation between the 3D best direction and location ( p Ͻ 0.001, r ϭ 0.64). When regressing the 2D best directions for the horizontal and frontal planes on unit location, highly significant correlations ( p Ͻ 0.001) were obtained for all three subjects in both planes, with r 2 -values ranging from 0.6 to 0.8 (plots not shown). Units located in the posterior part of the muscle were best activated for out–backward (horizontal plane) and out– downward (frontal plane) forces. As unit location changed to the more anterior part of the muscle, best directions gradually changed to the out–forward and out–upward directions, with no suggestion of discontinuity at posterior–medial or medi- al–anterior compartmental boundaries. The range of threshold magnitudes in our sample of BI and deltoid units extended from 0.2 to 28 N, and ϳ 50% of the units had threshold forces of Ͼ 12 N. Thus, a substantial number of units in both muscles were recruited at relatively high force levels and could be assumed to contract quickly enough to be involved in the generation of fast-rising forces or movements. Moreover, as will be explained below, we found that units with threshold magnitudes as low as 2 N were involved in the generation of fast pulses of isometric force. Figure 13 illustrates the results from an experiment in which subject D produced isometric pulses to different targets in the frontal plane. (This figure is also representative of the results of the second dynamic experiment.) Simultaneous recordings from two different electrodes are shown. The threshold lines and preferred directions (in the frontal plane) of two units found on the two different electrodes are shown in Figure 13 a . The best direction of unit MD (found in the center of deltoid, Fig. 13 b ) was almost straight outward with only a slight upward component. On the other hand, unit M/AD (found in a recording location ϳ 3 cm anterior to unit MD) had an up–forward best direction. Consider now the relative timing of the multiunit bursts on the two recording electrodes during dynamic pulses (with a 250 msec time to peak) in the three different directions (Fig. 13 c , d ). As the direction of the pulse changed from upward ( top lines ) to up–out ( bottom lines ), the bursts in the two recordings reversed their order. For the upward direction (closer to best direction of unit M/AD), the burst in the recording at the more anterior site was earlier. For the outward direction (closer to best direction of unit MD), the burst in the recording at the more posterior site was earlier. Because we have ascertained that units M/AD and MD are involved in those bursts (as marked in the three-trial unit rasters), it appears that the recruitment order of these individual units also changed depending on the direction of the dynamic force. We found that threshold data for 93% of our biceps and deltoid motor units could be fit with lines in 2D and /or planes in 3D, consistent with a model in which activation levels are tuned as a cosine f unction of force direction (Fig. 2) and in consonance with the well established idea that the descending inputs to spinal motoneuronal –interneuronal pools may have cosine-tuned activ- ity (Georgopoulos et al., 1982, 1988; Fortier et al., 1993). Using the orientation of the line or plane as a measure of the best direction of a unit, we showed that SMUs of the same muscle were not activated homogeneously but instead different units had different best directions. The best directions of units changed continuously with their locations in the muscle and did not cluster into distinct groups. For deltoid, the gradual change in best directions may echo the gradual change in the mechanical actions of the muscle fibers (Buneo et al., 1996). As discussed below, the gradual change of best directions in biceps might be understood by considering that during the time course of a movement, biceps units may act in synergy with various deltoid units. Thus, the directional specificity of the recruitment order may act in con- junction with the phenomenon of size-ordered recruitment, to enable smooth and fatigue-resistant muscle contractions. But before developing these ideas, we will consider several technical issues that influence the interpretation of our data. Using only a 2 df force transducer in the first set of experiments raised the concern that any significant differences between 2D best ...
Context 3
... data for one deltoid unit. The top three rows depict the threshold lines for each of the three planes. These were derived only from trials with force ramps within the respective plane. The bottom row shows the 3D plane fit to data from all trials. In 57 of the 59 units examined in this set of experiments the plane fit to the threshold data was significant ( p Ͻ 0.05). In 45 of these units the plane fit was significant at the p Ͻ 0.01 level. The values of the correlation coefficient r for the x , y , and z coordinates of the threshold force levels ranged from 0.40 to 0.95. These results illustrate that in consonance with the line fit to the 2D data, threshold data in 3D can be fit by a plane. In Figure 10, the threshold line for unit 10.1 in the sagittal plane was oriented much like that for unit 9.1, with a best direction in the down–backward direction. In the horizontal and frontal planes, the threshold line yielded a best direction of out–back- ward and out–downward, respectively. The orientation of the threshold plane in the bottom row illustrates that the 3D best direction of unit 10.1 was in the out–back–downward direction. Unit 10.2 was found in a recording location 6.5 cm anterior to that of unit 10.1 (i.e., in the medial deltoid). Its best direction in the sagittal plane was in the up–forward direction, comparable to that of unit 9.2. Considering only the sagittal plane, one might thus conclude that the best directions for units 10.1 and 10.2 differed by 180°. The threshold lines for the horizontal and frontal planes, however, show that the best directions for unit 10.2 in these planes differed from those of unit 10.1 by only 60 and 15°, respectively. With respect to the lines for unit 10.1, the threshold lines for unit 10.2 were slightly rotated counterclockwise, such that the best direction was out–forward for the horizontal plane and straight outward for the frontal plane. Accordingly, the plane fit to the 3D data yielded a best direction for unit 10.2 that was oriented out–forward and slightly up. Of the three units in Figure 10, unit 10.3 was found in the most anterior recording location. Considering only the sagittal plane, this unit appeared to have approximately the same best direction as unit 10.2; however, the threshold lines for the other two planes were rotated counterclockwise with respect to those of unit 10.2 (by 30 and 35° for the horizontal and frontal planes, respectively). The best directions for these planes are thus forward– out (with a greater forward component than that of unit 10.2) in the hori- zontal plane and out–up in the frontal plane. The orientation of the threshold plane in 3D confirms that the best direction of this unit is up – out–forward. It follows from these results that the very large (or very small) differences found between the best directions of deltoid units for forces in the sagittal plane are deceptive. Units with apparently opposite best directions in the sagittal plane (such as units 10.1 and 10.2) might in fact have threshold planes that differ only relatively little in their orientations. Thus, in 3D space the dif- ference between the best directions of units 10.1 and 10.2 was only 53°. On the other hand, units with approximately the same best direction in the sagittal plane might show significant differ- ences between their best directions in 3D. Thus, the difference between the 3D orientation of the planes of units 10.2 and 10.3 was 48°, although their threshold lines in the sagittal plane dif- fered by only 6°. Analogous to the bimodal units described above for biceps (e.g., unit 6.3) we also found evidence for multiple directional inputs on deltoid SMUs (19%, n ϭ 14 in the 2D experiment (Fig. 9), 7%, n ϭ 5 in the 3D experiments). The 3D recruitment data for a bidirectional deltoid unit are shown in Figure 11. From the sagittal plane data, which are best fit by two approximately par- allel threshold lines, one cannot discern whether the threshold planes in 3D would be parallel, yielding two best directions opposite to each other or whether the two planes would have a line of intersection. However, two parallel threshold planes would intersect all three experimental planes with two parallel lines, and the two-line fits to the horizontal and frontal plane data clearly show that this is not the case. The two lines in these plots are not parallel but instead intersect at an angle of 90° in the horizontal and 125° in the frontal plane. Accordingly, the two threshold planes fit to the 3D data have a line of intersection. The best directions for this unit are up – out–forward and down – out–back- ward and differ by 93° in 3D. None of the threshold data of bidirectional units found in the second set of experiments were fit by two parallel planes, meaning that all bidirectional units exhib- ited intersecting threshold planes. The differences between the two best directions of such units ranged from 58 to 96° in 3D space with an average value of 77°. In Figure 12, each deltoid SMU is represented by a unit vector radiating (from the origin) in the best direction of the unit. The color of the vector codes for the anterior–posterior location of the unit in the muscle. In general, as unit location changed from posterior (blue end of spectrum) to anterior (red end), best direction gradually changed from down –backward to up– forward. A similar pattern was found in all three subjects (Fig. 12 a ). Regressing recording location on the x , y , z coordinate of the tip of the unit vector yielded significant correlations for subject A ( p Ͻ 0.001, r ϭ 0.79) and subject B ( p Ͻ 0.05, r ϭ 0.71) but a nonsignificant correlation for subject D ( p ϭ 0.066, r ϭ 0.66). Because all subjects exhibited a qualitatively similar relation between best direction and location, we then pooled the data (Fig. 12 b ). Pooling data from all subjects by normalizing the unit location (across the width of each subject’s deltoid, see Fig. 13 b ) yielded a highly significant relation between the 3D best direction and location ( p Ͻ 0.001, r ϭ 0.64). When regressing the 2D best directions for the horizontal and frontal planes on unit location, highly significant correlations ( p Ͻ 0.001) were obtained for all three subjects in both planes, with r 2 -values ranging from 0.6 to 0.8 (plots not shown). Units located in the posterior part of the muscle were best activated for out–backward (horizontal plane) and out– downward (frontal plane) forces. As unit location changed to the more anterior part of the muscle, best directions gradually changed to the out–forward and out–upward directions, with no suggestion of discontinuity at posterior–medial or medi- al–anterior compartmental boundaries. The range of threshold magnitudes in our sample of BI and deltoid units extended from 0.2 to 28 N, and ϳ 50% of the units had threshold forces of Ͼ 12 N. Thus, a substantial number of units in both muscles were recruited at relatively high force levels and could be assumed to contract quickly enough to be involved in the generation of fast-rising forces or movements. Moreover, as will be explained below, we found that units with threshold magnitudes as low as 2 N were involved in the generation of fast pulses of isometric force. Figure 13 illustrates the results from an experiment in which subject D produced isometric pulses to different targets in the frontal plane. (This figure is also representative of the results of the second dynamic experiment.) Simultaneous recordings from two different electrodes are shown. The threshold lines and preferred directions (in the frontal plane) of two units found on the two different electrodes are shown in Figure 13 a . The best direction of unit MD (found in the center of deltoid, Fig. 13 b ) was almost straight outward with only a slight upward component. On the other hand, unit M/AD (found in a recording location ϳ 3 cm anterior to unit MD) had an up–forward best direction. Consider now the relative timing of the multiunit bursts on the two recording electrodes during dynamic pulses (with a 250 msec time to peak) in the three different directions (Fig. 13 c , d ). As the direction of the pulse changed from upward ( top lines ) to up–out ( bottom lines ), the bursts in the two recordings reversed their order. For the upward direction (closer to best direction of unit M/AD), the burst in the recording at the more anterior site was earlier. For the outward direction (closer to best direction of unit MD), the burst in the recording at the more posterior site was earlier. Because we have ascertained that units M/AD and MD are involved in those bursts (as marked in the three-trial unit rasters), it appears that the recruitment order of these individual units also changed depending on the direction of the dynamic force. We found that threshold data for 93% of our biceps and deltoid motor units could be fit with lines in 2D and /or planes in 3D, consistent with a model in which activation levels are tuned as a cosine f unction of force direction (Fig. 2) and in consonance with the well established idea that the descending inputs to spinal motoneuronal –interneuronal pools may have cosine-tuned activ- ity (Georgopoulos et al., 1982, 1988; Fortier et al., 1993). Using the orientation of the line or plane as a measure of the best direction of a unit, we showed that SMUs of the same muscle were not activated homogeneously but instead different units had different best directions. The best directions of units changed continuously with their locations in the muscle and did not cluster into distinct groups. For deltoid, the gradual change in best directions may echo the gradual change in the mechanical actions of the muscle fibers (Buneo et al., 1996). As discussed below, the gradual change of best directions in biceps might be understood by considering that during the time course of a movement, biceps units may act in synergy with various deltoid units. Thus, the directional ...
Context 4
... subserves an activation of part of the parent muscle in synergy with various other muscles or motor units in the system. The force vectors produced by such units might be necessary to balance the ones generated by other synergistic muscles during the production of a desired force (Flanders, 1993). Thus, the bidirectional units found here might constitute subpopulations that are activated in synergy with other units in the system. This idea is consistent with findings by Jongen et al. (1989) who report intramuscular differences in activation during cocontraction of other muscles. If these bimodal SMUs receive input from only unimodally tuned neurons, the question arises as to how unimodal inputs can produce bimodal outputs. Summing two cosine f unctions always results in a third unimodal cosine f unction. Even when the two input f unctions have a threshold nonlinearity (see Materials and Methods section of this study and Flanders and Soechting, 1990) their addition results in a unimodal output f unction, unless the angular separation between the two input peaks is wide and/or the threshold is high. Thus, to reproduce the tuning pattern of bimodal units whose two best directions differ by Ͻ 90° (the norm in our study), some kind of nonlinear interaction has to occur between the two unimodal inputs. Using NEURON simulation software (Hines and Carnevale, 1997) we attempted to create the observed MN output solely from a specific pattern of convergence of unimodally tuned inputs at the dendritic arbor of the MN (Herrmann, 1998). Several combinations of inputs with different relative locations, weights, and preferred directions were simulated, but none of them re- sulted in the pattern of bimodal output observed experimentally. Thus, it seems that a solution at the level of the motoneurons would have to be a very specific one and that unimodal inputs do not tend to produce a bimodal output as a result of simple convergence patterns. An alternative model might contain a re- ciprocal inhibition between two unimodally tuned inputs. It is also possible that the bidirectional tuning of SMUs arises from bimodally tuned inputs. It has been suggested that a muscle is organized into smaller submodules such that the elements controlled by the CNS are neuromuscular compartments rather than whole muscles (Wind- horst et al., 1989; English et al., 1993). Such partitioned control might be functionally advantageous because individual muscles are nonuniform in their mechanical actions (Chanaud et al., 1991a; Carrasco and English, 1997) and/or their fiber type dis- tribution (Chanaud et al., 1991b). The existence of neuromuscu- lar compartments suggests the fractionation of the MN pool of a muscle into smaller, differently activated subpopulations. Presum- ably each subpopulation would then receive homogenous activa- tion such that recruitment order within each compartment is determined by MN size (Windhorst et al., 1989). For cat hindlimb muscle, the fibers innervated by MNs of the same subpopulation have been found to be largely restricted to a distinct muscular territory (English and Letbetter, 1982). According to these arguments we would have expected BI and deltoid units to fall into a limited number of groups, each group containing units with the same best direction and localized in a restricted muscle area. Instead we found a continuous distribu- tion of best directions with no convincing evidence for clustering. When grouping BI units with similar best directions together, their anatomical territories overlapped widely and spanned the whole width of the muscle (Fig. 8). Thus, the MN pool of these muscles appears not to be compartmentalized but to instead receive a more continuously distributed innervation. Georgopou- los et al. (1988) have shown that the 3D best directions of motor cortical cells uniformly cover the 3D space and do not appear to cluster into distinct groups. Thus, the neural substrate to create a continuous, finely graded distribution of the best directions of SMUs is available at the supraspinal level. A similar lack of compartmentalization has been found in a bitendoned finger extensor (Schieber et al., 1997). Although one might logically expect to find two distinct compartments in this muscle (one for each tendon), most units contribute to force on both tendons, with selectivity for either tendon ranging on a continuum. Analogously, BI has two distinct anatomical subdivi- sions with different attachments and moment arms at the shoulder (Wood et al., 1989), but the organization of its MN pool reveals more of a continuum. In deltoid, on the other hand, it seems apparent that the pulling directions of the SMUs change contin- ually with location along the broad attachment of the muscle (Buneo et al., 1997), thus motivating the continuously changing directional innervation of different parts of the muscle. Accordingly, the best directions of deltoid SMUs varied continuously across a wide area of space (Fig. 12). The correlation between best direction and recording location might also be related to the topography of the spinal M N pool as suggested by Kernell (1989). A rostrocaudal topography has been described for M N pools of the two parts of deltoid muscle in the rat (Choi and Hoover, 1996). The considerable overlap found between these two pools may be consistent with our finding of continuous rather than compartmentalized preferred directions and multiple directional preferences for some units. Similar features of muscle activation are found in movements and dynamic isometric forces (Ghez and Gordon, 1987; Flanders et al., 1996; Pellegrini and Flanders, 1996). This suggests that similar control strategies are involved in these two tasks. In Figure 13 we show that the timing of the activity of a unit moved from an early to a progressively later point in the force pulse as the direction of the pulse moved away from the best direction of the unit. Given the parallels between the neural control of force pulses and movements, one can speculate that the relative timing of recruitment during a movement could potentially be predicted from the best directions of the units under static conditions and the directions of dynamic forces during movement. As arm muscles change their mechanical actions with arm posture, the directional tuning of whole muscles generally changes in parallel with the new pulling directions of the muscles (Flanders and Soechting, 1990). Although it remains to be determined whether the best directions of the motor units change with posture (or if whole muscle tuning changes via recruitment of different units), this suggests that the central control mechanism takes limb configuration into account when issuing motor commands. Muscles with different mechanical actions are activated at different times during a reaching movement (Flanders et al., 1996). Thus, a factor determining the timing of the activation of a muscle might be the degree of correspondence between its pulling direction and the direction of force currently required. If differences in mechanical action exist across units within a muscle, the population of units optimally fit to produce a required force vector will change with limb position and force direction during a movement. Based on this logic, we hypothesize that at different points in the movement, the various units are recruited according to their mechanical actions. The neuromuscular system could thereby gradually make use of the wide number of units available instead of activating a few discrete populations. This spatiotemporal recruitment rule would necessarily involve violations of the size principle (Henneman et al., 1965; see also DeL uca and Erim, 1994). However, reminiscent of the advantages of size-ordered recruitment, this strategy would minimize fatigue and increase efficiency because mainly those units ideally suited for the production of a required force vector would be active at any given time. Furthermore, because cosine-tuned units are active over a wide range of directions, the change in unit activation during a movement need not consist in the abrupt recruitment and derecruitment of distinct subpopulations but can instead proceed smoothly by gradual recruitment and derecruitment and differential modulation of the firing rates of different units. This smooth change in relative unit activity would agree with the postulate that in the execution of a motor act, the control mechanism strives to minimize abrupt changes in the motor commands (Dornay et al., ...

Similar publications

Article
Full-text available
After exposure of the human body to resistive exercise, the force‐generation capacity of the trained muscles increases significantly. Despite decades of research, the neural and muscular stimuli that initiate these changes in muscle force are not yet fully understood. The study of these adaptations is further complicated by the fact that the change...

Citations

... We show here that most of the motor units from the triceps surae muscles might receive two sources of common inputs. The combination of multiple independent inputs might therefore explain why previous works reported changes in recruitment strategies between motor units from the same muscle while changing the mechanical constraints of the task (Desnedt & Gidaux, 1981;Herrmann & Flanders, 1998;Marshall et al., 2022;ter Haar Romeny et al., 1984). ...
Article
Full-text available
Recent studies have suggested that the nervous system generates movements by controlling groups of motor neurons (synergies) that do not always align with muscle anatomy. In this study, we determined whether these synergies are robust across tasks with different mechanical constraints. We identified motor neuron synergies using principal component analysis (PCA) and cross‐correlations between smoothed discharge rates of motor neurons. In part 1, we used simulations to validate these methods. The results suggested that PCA can accurately identify the number of common inputs and their distribution across active motor neurons. Moreover, the results confirmed that cross‐correlation can separate pairs of motor neurons that receive common inputs from those that do not receive common inputs. In part 2, 16 individuals performed plantarflexion at three ankle angles while we recorded EMG signals from the gastrocnemius lateralis (GL) and medialis (GM) and the soleus (SOL) with grids of surface electrodes. The PCA revealed two motor neuron synergies. These motor neuron synergies were relatively stable, with no significant differences in the distribution of motor neuron weights across ankle angles (P = 0.62). When the cross‐correlation was calculated for pairs of motor units tracked across ankle angles, we observed that only 13.0% of pairs of motor units from GL and GM exhibited significant correlations of their smoothed discharge rates across angles, confirming the low level of common inputs between these muscles. Overall, these results highlight the modularity of movement control at the motor neuron level, suggesting a sensible reduction of computational resources for movement control. image Key points The CNS might generate movements by activating groups of motor neurons (synergies) with common inputs. We show here that two main sources of common inputs drive the motor neurons innervating the triceps surae muscles during isometric ankle plantarflexions. We report that the distribution of these common inputs is globally invariant despite changing the mechanical constraints of the tasks, i.e. the ankle angle. These results suggest the functional relevance of the modular organization of the CNS to control movements.
... Optimality suggests flexibly using MUs suited to each situation [5][6][7] . However, nearly a century of research supports an alternative rigid strategy that approximates optimality 1,8,9 . ...
... Second, cell-intrinsic mechanisms cause some MUs to rotate on and off, over many seconds, to combat fatigue 27 . Finally, for 'multifunctional' muscles with multiple mechanical actions, the size principle holds only within each action 6,28,29 . Each mechanical action is hypothesized to have its own common drive (or 'synergy'). ...
... We can fit this model with one latent (x t can be a single value, as for all analyses in Fig. 7) or multiple latents (Fig. 8b). The relationship between the MU activity and the underlying latent(s) is given by (6) where f i denotes a learned link function for the i th MU, and τ i denotes a learned lag between its response and the shared latent variables. We constrained τ i ∈ [− 25,25] ms. ...
Article
Full-text available
Voluntary movement requires communication from cortex to the spinal cord, where a dedicated pool of motor units (MUs) activates each muscle. The canonical description of MU function rests upon two foundational tenets. First, cortex cannot control MUs independently but supplies each pool with a common drive. Second, MUs are recruited in a rigid fashion that largely accords with Henneman’s size principle. Although this paradigm has considerable empirical support, a direct test requires simultaneous observations of many MUs across diverse force profiles. In this study, we developed an isometric task that allowed stable MU recordings, in a rhesus macaque, even during rapidly changing forces. Patterns of MU activity were surprisingly behavior-dependent and could be accurately described only by assuming multiple drives. Consistent with flexible descending control, microstimulation of neighboring cortical sites recruited different MUs. Furthermore, the cortical population response displayed sufficient degrees of freedom to potentially exert fine-grained control. Thus, MU activity is flexibly controlled to meet task demands, and cortex may contribute to this ability.
... In particular, the biceps brachii, used in this study, is known to have multiple anatomical neuromuscular compartments, with separate subdivisions even within the gross anatomical divide of the short and long head 45 . Biceps brachii motor unit recruitment has been shown to vary depending on the contraction levels in the flexion and/or supination directions, with motor units distributed across the biceps with no clear spatial distribution relevant to function [30][31][32]46 . Other muscles have been shown to have similar taskdependent recruitment order differences, such as in the first dorsal interosseus muscle when performing flexion versus abduction of the index finger and in a variety of non-multifunctional arm muscles 33,47 . ...
Article
Full-text available
Brain-machine interfaces (BMIs) have the potential to augment human functions and restore independence in people with disabilities, yet a compromise between non-invasiveness and performance limits their relevance. Here, we hypothesized that a non-invasive neuromuscular-machine interface (NMI) providing real-time neurofeedback of individual motor units within a muscle could enable independent motor unit control to an extent suitable for high-performance BMI applications. Over 6 days of training, 8 participants progressively learned to skillfully and independently control three biceps brachii motor units to complete a two-dimensional center-out task. We show that neurofeedback enabled motor unit activity that largely violated recruitment constraints observed during ramp-and-hold isometric contractions thought to limit individual motor unit controllability. Finally, participants demonstrated the suitability of individual motor units for powering general applications through a spelling task. These results illustrate the flexibility of the sensorimotor system and highlight individual motor units as a promising source of control for BMI applications.
... These task groups may have intrinsic properties optimized for the performance of a specific functional task (Hoffer et al. 1987), however, recent studies suggest that the physical location of the units may also be important (Hodson-Tole and Wakeling 2009). Given that individual muscles are nonuniform in their mechanical actions (Chanaud et al. 1991), directional tuning of single motor units has been observed (Herrmann and Flanders 1998). The directional tuning of single motor units seems to depend more upon the location of motor units within the muscle volume rather than on the degree of muscle activation (Herrmann and Flanders 1998). ...
... Given that individual muscles are nonuniform in their mechanical actions (Chanaud et al. 1991), directional tuning of single motor units has been observed (Herrmann and Flanders 1998). The directional tuning of single motor units seems to depend more upon the location of motor units within the muscle volume rather than on the degree of muscle activation (Herrmann and Flanders 1998). ...
Article
Full-text available
The purpose of this study is to investigate whether regional modulation of the ankle plantarflexors during standing was related to the recruitment of motor units associated with force direction. Fourteen participants performed a multi-directional leaning task in standing. Participants stood on a force platform and maintained their center of pressure in five different target directions. Motor unit firings were extracted by decomposition of high-density surface electromyograms recorded from the ankle plantarflexor muscles. The motor unit barycentre, defined as the weighted mean of the maximal average rectified values across columns and rows, was used to evaluate the medio-lateral and proximo-distal changes in the surface representation of single motor units across different leaning target directions. Using a motor unit tracking analysis, groups of motor units were identified as being common or unique across the target directions. The leaning directions had an effect on the spatial representations of motor units in the medial gastrocnemius and soleus (p < 0.05), but not in the lateral gastrocnemius (p > 0.05). Motor unit action potentials were represented in the medial and proximal aspects of the muscles during forward vs. lateral leans. Further analysis determined that the common motor units were found in similar spatial locations across the target directions, whereas newly recruited unique motor units were found in different spatial locations according to target direction (p < 0.05). The central nervous system may possess the ability to activate different groups of motor units according to task demands to meet the force-direction requirements of the leaning task.
... CC-BY-NC-ND 4.0 International license available under a (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is made 15,25,36,37 . Other muscles have been shown to have similar task-dependent recruitment order differences, such as in the first dorsal interosseus muscle when performing flexion versus abduction of the index finger and in a variety of non-multifunctional arm muscles 20,38 . ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Brain-machine interfaces (BMIs) have the potential to restore independence in people with disabilities, yet a compromise between non-invasiveness and performance limits their translational relevance. Here, we demonstrate a high-performance BMI controlled by individual motor units non-invasively recorded from the biceps brachii. Through real-time auditory and visual neurofeedback of motor unit activity, 8 participants learned to skillfully and independently control three motor units in order to complete a two-dimensional center-out task, with marked improvements in control over 6 days of training. Concomitantly, dimensionality of the motor unit population increased significantly relative to naturalistic behaviors, largely violating recruitment orders displayed during stereotyped, isometric muscle contractions. Finally, participants' performance on a spelling task demonstrated translational potential of a motor unit BMI, exceeding performance across existing non-invasive BMIs. These results demonstrate a yet-unexplored level of flexibility of the peripheral sensorimotor system and show that this can be exploited to create novel non-invasive, high-performance BMIs.
... During cycling, effective muscle coordination is required to provide an effective force profile, given that activation patterns can affect the direction (Herrmann and Flanders, 1998), magnitude and duration (Blake and Wakeling, 2015) of the external force developed. ...
Thesis
Full-text available
Optimisation of movement strategies during cycling is an area which has gathered a lot of attention over the past decade. Resolutions to augment performance have involved manipulations of bicycle mechanics, including chainring geometries. Elliptical chainrings are proposed to provide a greater effective diameter during the downstroke, manipulating mechanical leverage and resulting in greater power production during this period. A review of the literature indicates that there is a pervasive gap in our understanding of how the theoretical underpinnings of elliptical chainrings might be translated to practical use. Despite reasonable theory of how these chainrings might enforce a variation in crank angular velocity and consequently alter force production, performance-based analyses have struggled to present evidence of this. The purpose of this thesis was to provide a novel approach to this problem by combining experimental data with musculoskeletal modelling and evaluating how elliptical chainrings might influence crank reactive forces, joint kinematics, muscle-tendon unit behaviour and muscle activation. One main study was proposed to execute this analysis, and an anatomically constrained model was subsequently used to determine the joint kinematics and muscle-tendon unit behaviour. Bespoke elliptical chainrings were designed for this study and as such, different levels of chainring eccentricity (i.e. ratio of major to minor axis) and positioning against the crank were presented whilst controlling the influence of other variables known to affect the neuromuscular system such as cadence and load. Findings presented in this thesis makes a new and major contribution in our understanding of the neuromusculoskeletal adaptations which occur when using elliptical chainrings, showing alterations in crank reaction force, muscle-tendon unit velocities, joint kinematics and muscle excitation over a range of cadences and loads, and provides direction for where the future of this research might be best applied. Keywords: Elliptical chainrings; Cycling; Musculoskeletal modelling; Principal Component Analysis; Electromyography
... Even though there seems to be a size rule governing the recuritment of MUs within a pool (Henneman et al. 1965), different studies on human subjects observed Communicated by Toshio Moritani. that MUs in the same upper limb muscle may be activated selectively (Herrmann and Flanders 1998;Riek and Bawa 1992;ter Romeny et al. 1984). Moreover, the selective activation of different muscle sub-portions (Brown et al. 2007;Holtermann et al. 2009;Pappas et al. 2002) further suggests that MUs may be recruited according to functional demands imposed by the motor task and, thus, according to their location within the muscle. ...
... Different motor units were recruited during different tasks. This observation is consistent with previous studies (Herrmann and Flanders 1998;Riek and Bawa 1992;ter Romeny et al. 1984) that identified task-specific recruitment of MUs of upper limb muscles. However, the different units, recruited during different tasks, were not identified to be spatially localized in different portions of the BB during different tasks. ...
... However, the different units, recruited during different tasks, were not identified to be spatially localized in different portions of the BB during different tasks. This observation is in line with the study performed by Herrmann and Flanders (1998), who demonstrated that MUs with closely located territories may have different directional tuning. Our results, however, differ from those reported by ter Haar Romeny and collaborators (1984), who observed a preferential recruitment of units in the most lateral and medial regions of BB long head during elbow flexion and forearm supination, respectively. ...
Article
Full-text available
PurposeDifferent motor units (MUs) in the biceps brachii (BB) muscle have been shown to be preferentially recruited during either elbow flexion or supination. Whether these different units reside within different regions is an open issue. In this study, we tested wheter MUs recruited during submaximal isometric tasks of elbow flexion and supination for two contraction levels and with the wrist fixed at two different angles are spatially localized in different BB portions.Methods The MUs’ firing instants were extracted by decomposing high-density surface electromyograms (EMG), detected from the BB muscle of 12 subjects with a grid of electrodes (4 rows along the BB longitudinal axis, 16 columns medio-laterally). The firing instants were then used to trigger and average single-differential EMGs. The average rectified value was computed separately for each signal and the maximal value along each column in the grid was retained. The center of mass, defined as the weighted mean of the maximal, average rectified value across columns, was then consdiered to assess the medio-lateral changes in the MU surface representation between conditions.ResultsContraction level, but neither wrist position nor force direction (flexion vs. supination), affected the spatial distribution of BB MUs. In particular, higher forces were associated with the recruitment of BB MUs whose action potentials were represented more medially.Conclusion Although the action potentials of BB MUs were represented locally across the muscle medio-lateral region, dicrimination between elbow flexion or supination seems unlikely from the surface representation of MUs action potentials.
... The solutions available within a muscle may, therefore, be constrained by several factors. For example: organization into motor unit task groups (20); common neural drive acting across motor unit populations (21) and; spatial organization of motor units influencing which can appropriately contribute to the direction of the force vector required at the joint (22)(23)(24). As such, it may be that although the number of solutions available to the limb as a whole is limited by pedal cycle duration, reductions in the solutions available within individual muscles may not occur. ...
Article
Full-text available
Purpose: A key determinant of muscle coordination and maximum power output during cycling is pedalling cadence. During cycling the neuromuscular system may select from numerous solutions that solve the task demands while producing the same result. For more challenging tasks fewer solutions will be available. Changes in the variability of individual muscle excitations (EMG) and multi-muscle coordination, quantified by entropic half-life (EnHL), can reflect the number of solutions available at each system level. We therefore ask whether reduced variability in muscle coordination patterns occur at critical cadences and if they coincide with reduced variability in excitations of individual muscles. Methods: Eleven trained cyclists completed an array of cadence-power output conditions. EnHL of EMG intensity recorded from 10 leg muscles and EnHL of principal components describing muscle coordination were calculated. Multivariate adaptive regressive splines were used to determine the relationships between each EnHL and cycling condition or excitation characteristics (duration, duty cycle). Results: Muscle coordination became more persistent at cadences up to 120 r.p.m., indicated by increasing EnHL values. Changes in EnHL at the level of the individual muscles differed from the changes in muscle coordination EnHL, with longer EnHLs occurring at the slowest (< 80 r.p.m.) and fastest (>120 r.p.m.) cadences. EnHL of the main power producing muscles however reached a minimum by 80 r.p.m. and did not change across the faster cadences studied. Conclusions: Muscle coordination patterns, rather than the contribution of individual muscles, are key to power production at faster cadences in trained cyclists. Reductions in maximum power output at cadences above 120 r.p.m. could be a function of the time available to coordinate orientation and transfer of forces to the pedals.
... Such selective activation of muscles subvolumes implies that motor neurons serving different subvolumes receive distinct net inputs. Following this reasoning, it is possible that different pools of MUs, each elicited for a specific purpose (e.g., to regulate force direction or to endure a fatiguing contraction 13,31,32 ), receive different inputs. The present results suggest this concept may be extended, at least in VM, to motor neurons serving different proximodistal muscle regions. ...
Article
Introduction: Previous evidence suggests the fibres of different motor units reside within distinct vastus medialis (VM) regions. Whether the activity of these motor units may be modulated differently remains unknown. Here we assess the discharge rate of motor units detected proximo-distally from VM to address this issue. Methods: Surface electromyograms (EMGs) were recorded proximally and distally from VM while ten healthy subjects performed isometric contractions. Single motor units were decomposed from surface EMGs. The smoothed discharge rates of motor units identified from the same and from different VM regions were then cross-correlated. Results: During low-level contractions, the discharge rate varied more similarly for distal (cross-correlation peak; interquartile interval: 0.27-0.40) and proximal (0.28-0.52) than for proximo-distal pairs of VM motor units (0.20-0.33; P=0.006). Discussion: The discharge rates of motor units from different proximo-distal VM regions show less similarity in their variations than those of pairs of units either distally or proximally. This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.
... For different skeletal muscles, regional changes in the EMG amplitude have been observed for a number of circumstances, such as for different joint positions (Watanabe et al., 2012), contraction durations (Farina et al., 2008;Tucker et al., 2009) and force levels (Holtermann et al., 2005;Rojas-Martínez et al., 2012), as well as during standing (Vieira et al., 2010) or dynamic contractions (Falla and Farina, 2007). The mechanical efficiency of specific regions within the muscle (Herrmann and Flanders, 1998;Holtermann et al., 2005) and neural strategies for delaying and/or minimizing muscle fatigue (Falla and Farina, 2007;van Dieën et al., 1993) are examples of potential mechanisms of uneven distribution of activity within muscles. ...
Article
Proper muscle activity quantification is highly relevant to monitor and treat spastic cocontraction. As activity may distribute unevenly within muscle volumes, particularly for pennate calf muscles, surface electromyograms (EMGs) detected by traditional bipolar montage may provide biased estimations of muscle activity. We compared cocontraction estimates obtained using bipolar vs grids of electrodes (high-density EMG, HD-EMG). EMGs were collected from medial gastrocnemius, soleus and tibialis anterior during isometric plantar and dorsi-flexion efforts at three levels (30%, 70% and 100% MVC), knee flexed and extended. Cocontraction index (CCI) was estimated separately for each electrode pair in the grid. While soleus and tibialis anterior CCI estimates did not depend on the detection system considered, for gastrocnemius bipolar electrodes provided larger cocontraction estimates than HD-EMG at highest effort levels, at both knee angles (ANOVA; P < .001). Interestingly, HD-EMG detected greater gastrocnemius EMGs distally during plantar flexions, and greater CCI values proximally during dorsiflexions. These results suggest that bipolar electrodes: (i) provide reliable estimates of soleus and tibialis anterior cocontraction; (ii) may under-or overestimate gastrocnemius cocontraction, depending on their distal or proximal position.