Figure 4 - uploaded by Sky Onosson
Content may be subject to copyright.
Vowel formants before /z/ vs. /z ʕ / (a) and after /z/ vs. /z ʕ / (b). Formants plotted are mean values in adjacent third of the vowel. Ellipses show 95% confidence intervals (omitted where low token count prohibits calculation).

Vowel formants before /z/ vs. /z ʕ / (a) and after /z/ vs. /z ʕ / (b). Formants plotted are mean values in adjacent third of the vowel. Ellipses show 95% confidence intervals (omitted where low token count prohibits calculation).

Source publication
Preprint
Full-text available
PREPRINT: This paper provides an acoustic description of /z/ and /zʕ/ in Tŝilhqot’in (Northern Dene). These sounds are noted by Cook (1993, 2013) to show lenition and some degree of laterality in coda position. Based on recordings made in 2014 with a single, mother-tongue speaker of Tŝilhqot’in, we describe their acoustic properties and examine t...

Contexts in source publication

Context 1
... 3 provides kernel density estimate plots of /z/ and /z ʕ / distributions by mean F1 (yaxis) and F2 (x-axis), as measured within the consonants. Overall, Figure 3 shows that the two consonants differ substantially along F2 but much less so along F1; this is also the case for the vowels adjacent to /z/ and /z ʕ / (Figure 4), similar to Zawaydeh & de Jong's (2011) findings in Ammani-Jordanian Arabic. This indicates that, unlike in other languages (see Al Tamini (2017) on Arabic and Shahin (1997) on St'át'imcets), what is termed "pharyngealization" in Tŝilhqot'in is manifested primarily as backing, without substantial lowering. ...
Context 2
... predicted based on Cook (2013) and also described for Arabic ( Embarki et al. 2011, Laver 1994, adjacent vowels also reliably correlate with phonemic pharyngealization. For example, the final /a/ of teẑilhchaz /tez ʕ iɬt ͡ ʃaz/ 'I started to fry it' is realized as [ae] (with a relatively high F2) whereas in telhant'aẑ /teɬant'az ʕ / 'crowberry' it is realized as [ɑ] (with a relatively low F2). 10 Figure 4 provides the acoustic vowel spaces before /z/ vs. /z ʕ / (a) and after /z/ vs. /z ʕ / (b). This plot is based on F1 and F2 measurements averaged over the third of the vowel that is adjacent to the target consonant, i.e. the last third for vowels preceding /z/ and /z ʕ / and the first third for vowels occurring after /z/ and /z ʕ /. ...

Similar publications

Article
Full-text available
Teachers making errors in explanation that require subsequent self-correction is presumably common in education. However, it may be difficult to capture in research. In this study, teacher self-correction in the context of early childhood science education within a fictive frame was captured on video when documenting science activities over a prolo...
Article
Full-text available
This paper provides an acoustic description of /z/ and /z ʕ / in Tŝilhqot’in (Northern Dene). These sounds are noted by Cook (1993, 2013) to show lenition and some degree of laterality in coda position. Based on recordings made in 2014 with a single, mother-tongue speaker of Tŝilhqot’in, we describe their acoustic properties and examine their distr...
Article
Full-text available
This paper concerns the acquisition of the sign lexicon in L2 learners of Swedish Sign Language. Sampled data (conversation and narrative retelling) from a longitudinal learner corpus with 16 adult L2 signers was analyzed and compared with data from nine L1 signers. The use of three broad types of signs was analyzed: lexical signs, partly-lexical s...
Article
Full-text available
In second language research, the concept of cross-linguistic influence or transfer has frequently been used to describe the interaction between the first language (L1) and second language (L2) in the L2 acquisition process. However, less is known about the L2 acquisition of a sign language in general and specifically the differences in the acquisit...