Figure 2 - uploaded by Edo Ronald de Kloet
Content may be subject to copyright.
Quantitative morphological analysis of dendritic length. A , Representative photographs of Golgi-stained CA1 pyramidal neurons accompanied by typical drawing taken from adult rats that received low (left) versus high (right) amounts of LG in infancy. Apical dendrites on top (blue) and basal dendrites at the bottom (green) are delineated by a dotted line. Yellow marks represent typical locations where spine density was assessed. Drawings are based on three-dimensional stacks of Golgi-stained cells allowing accurate distinction between the cell of interest and neighboring cells. Note that the length of dendritic trees (both basal and apical dendrites) is significantly shorter in low LG ( n ϭ 6) compared with high LG ( n ϭ 7) offspring. Magnification, 40 ϫ . Scale bars, 200 ␮ m. B , Representative photographs depicting spine density for low (left) and high (right) LG offspring. Basal dendrites at the top and apical dendrites at the bottom are delineated by a dotted line. Note that spine density is lower in offspring of low LG ( n ϭ 6) compared with high LG ( n ϭ 7) mothers. Magnification, 630 ϫ . Scale bars, 2.5 ␮ m. From each cell, two 20 ␮ m segments from the apical dendrite and one 20 ␮ m segment from basal dendrite were analyzed (see yellow marks for typical locations in A ). 

Quantitative morphological analysis of dendritic length. A , Representative photographs of Golgi-stained CA1 pyramidal neurons accompanied by typical drawing taken from adult rats that received low (left) versus high (right) amounts of LG in infancy. Apical dendrites on top (blue) and basal dendrites at the bottom (green) are delineated by a dotted line. Yellow marks represent typical locations where spine density was assessed. Drawings are based on three-dimensional stacks of Golgi-stained cells allowing accurate distinction between the cell of interest and neighboring cells. Note that the length of dendritic trees (both basal and apical dendrites) is significantly shorter in low LG ( n ϭ 6) compared with high LG ( n ϭ 7) offspring. Magnification, 40 ϫ . Scale bars, 200 ␮ m. B , Representative photographs depicting spine density for low (left) and high (right) LG offspring. Basal dendrites at the top and apical dendrites at the bottom are delineated by a dotted line. Note that spine density is lower in offspring of low LG ( n ϭ 6) compared with high LG ( n ϭ 7) mothers. Magnification, 630 ϫ . Scale bars, 2.5 ␮ m. From each cell, two 20 ␮ m segments from the apical dendrite and one 20 ␮ m segment from basal dendrite were analyzed (see yellow marks for typical locations in A ). 

Source publication
Article
Full-text available
Maternal licking and grooming (LG) in infancy influences stress responsiveness and cognitive performance in the offspring. We examined the effects of variation in the frequency of pup LG on morphological, electrophysiological, and behavioral aspects of hippocampal synaptic plasticity under basal and stress-like conditions. We found shorter dendriti...

Contexts in source publication

Context 1
... predicts the risk for psy- chopathology over the lifespan (Oakley Browne et al., 1995; Kendler et al., 2002; McEwen, 2003; Nemeroff, 2004a,b; Coldwell et al., 2006). Studies in the rat suggest direct effects of maternal care on the development of neural systems that regulate cognitive, emotional, and neuroendocrine responses to stress (Meaney, 2001) and thus influence vulnerability for mood disorders. In the rat, naturally occurring variations in the frequency of pup licking and grooming (LG) provided by the dam during the first week of life are associated with individual differences in stress responsiveness, emotionality, and cognitive functioning in adult offspring. As adults, offspring of low compared with high LG mothers show increased hypothalamus-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) responses to stress (Liu et al., 1997; Weaver et al., 2004, 2005), enhanced emotionality (Caldji et al., 1998; Menard and Hakvoort, 2007), and impaired performance in tests of spatial learning and object recognition (Liu et al., 2000; Bredy et al., 2003, 2004; Toki et al., 2007). These effects are essentially reversed with cross fostering, suggesting a direct effect of maternal care (Francis et al., 1999; Caldji et al., 2003). The maternal effects on behavioral and HPA responses to stress depend on, at least in part, epigenetic programming of gene expression (Meaney and Szyf, 2005; Diorio and Meaney, 2007). However, such effects, and especially those on hippocampal- dependent forms of learning and memory, may also involve differences in synaptic plasticity. Indeed, maternal care alters the expression of synaptophysin, growth factors, and selected glutamate receptor subunits (Liu et al., 2000; Bredy et al., 2003, 2004; Toki et al., 2007). However, potential effects on the structure and function of hippocampal neurons have not been examined and are of particular relevance in light of studies suggesting parental effects on hippocampal volume in humans (Vythilingam et al., 2002; Buss et al., 2007) and of consistent reports of decreased hippocampal volume in patients with mood disorders (Sheline et al., 1996; Bremner et al., 2000). In the studies reported here, we examined the effects of variation in pup LG on the morphology of CA1 hippocampal neurons and on synaptic plasticity within this region in response to corticosteroid treatments that mimic basal versus stressful conditions (i.e., examining hippocampal function under a range of environmental conditions). Specifically, we investigated effects of maternal care on the following: (1) the morphology of CA1 neurons under resting condition, (2) LTP under low and high corticosterone (CORT) conditions ( in vitro ), (3) behavioral (learning/memory) performance in a fear conditioning task (high stress, in vivo ), and (4) levels of hippocampal mineralocorticoid receptor (MR) and glucocorticoid receptor (GR) expression. The frequency of the maternal behaviors is listed in Table 1. In addition to differing in the percentage of LG averaged over the first week of life, dams used in the current study also differed in the percentage of flat-back nursing (a form of passive maternal care) with low LG dams displaying significantly more flat-back nursing than high LG dams ( t ϭ 2.78; df (30) , p Ͻ 0.01, unpaired t test). High LG dams displayed significantly more LG than low LG dams across all days of the observation period ( F (1,21) ϭ 145.11; p Ͻ 0.0001, two-way ANOVA, repeated measures) (Fig. 1 B ). Furthermore, the frequency of maternal LG decreased over days across both groups (uncorrected, F (5,105) ϭ 3.10, p Ͻ 0.05; H-F corrected, F (3.8,79.7) ϭ 3.10; ␧ ϭ 0.759; p Ͻ 0.05). These findings are in agreement with previous reports and support the view that, in general, low LG dams display a more passive maternal care style than high LG dams (Champagne et al., 2003; Menard and Hakvoort, 2007). Enduring effects of variations in maternal care on hippocampal development have been suggested to occur via mechanisms similar to those associated with experience-dependent neural development (for review, see Meaney, 2001). This raises the question of whether early sensory experiences such as maternal LG (a form of tactile stimulation) could stably alter dendritic morphology and/or spine density during a critical period of development. To address this question, we used Golgi staining, a sensitive method allowing accurate assessment of morphological aspects of hippocampal neurons. We report morphological alterations in the length of both basal ( t ϭ 3.056; df ϭ 11; p Ͻ 0.01, unpaired t test) and apical ( t ϭ 2.38; df ϭ 11; p Ͻ 0.05, unpaired t test) CA1 pyramidal cell dendrites that were significantly shorter in adult offspring of low versus high LG mothers (Fig. 2 A , Table 2). Spine density (as determined in 20 ␮ m segments) of apical ( t ϭ 2.88; df ϭ 11; p Ͻ 0.05, unpaired t test) and basal ( t ϭ 2.57; df ϭ 11; p Ͻ 0.05, unpaired t test) dendrites was also significantly lower in low versus high LG offspring (Fig. 2 B , Table 2). Although no significant differences were observed in the number of dendritic branches (data not shown) as a function of maternal care, a trend toward statistical significance for the number of branching points was observed between offspring of low and high LG mothers, with low LG offspring displaying a trend toward fewer branching points on apical ( t ϭ 1.97; df ϭ 11, p Ͼ 0.05, unpaired t test) but not basal ( t ϭ 0.91; df ϭ 11; p Ͼ 0.05, n.s. unpaired t test) dendrites than high LG offspring (Table 2). Previous reports showed that as early as P8, there is decreased expression of genes encoding NMDA receptor (NR) subunits (i.e., NR2A and NR2B, particularly in the CA1 subfield) in the offspring of low compared with high LG mothers (Liu et al., 2000; Bredy et al., 2004). These characteristics endure into adulthood and correlate with impaired cognitive performance in hippocampal-dependent tasks (e.g., Morris water maze, object recognition) (Liu et al., 2000; Bredy et al., 2003). Therefore, we induced tetanic stimulation of Schaffer collaterals in hippocampal slices taken from low and high LG rats, which were bathed with either VEH or CORT and recorded synaptic activity for the next 60 min afterward. We report a significant interaction between maternal care (low versus high LG) and treatment (VEH versus CORT; F (1,8) ϭ 60.31; p Ͻ 0.001, two-way ANOVA, repeated measure). Sidak post hoc comparisons revealed dramatic differences after tetanic stimulation as a function of maternal care ( p Ͻ 0.001). Although synaptic potentiation was very prominent in rats from high LG mothers, no significant synaptic potentiation was seen 60 min after tetanic stimulation (10 Hz; 900 pulses) in offspring from low LG mothers (Fig. 3). It is noteworthy that the maximal responses evoked by low-frequency stimulation were significantly lower in offspring of low LG versus high LG mothers ( t ϭ 3.59; df ϭ 17; p Ͻ 0.01, unpaired t test), whereas half-maximum stimulus intensity values were comparable between both groups (Table 3). These findings suggest alterations in pre- and post-LTP synaptic transmission properties between the adult offspring of high and low LG mothers and reinforce the suggestion that differential synaptic transmission might be re- lated to differences in morphological aspects of CA1 neurons reported above. To ascertain that the effects reported above were not attributable to different levels of circulating basal CORT and ACTH, trunk blood samples were taken at rest when animals were killed by decapitation for either Golgi analysis (cohort 1: low LG, n ϭ 6; high LG, n ϭ 7), glucocorticoid receptor system analysis (cohort 2: low LG, n ϭ 5; high LG, n ϭ 5), or electrophysiology (cohort 3: low LG, n ϭ 10; high LG, n ϭ 11). For all cohorts, we report that CORT and ACTH levels did not differ under resting condition between low and high LG offspring. All values for CORT and ACTH levels are reported in Table 4 for each individual cohort. Stress and high CORT are known modulators of hippocampal functioning during learning and memory. CORT exerts its action via binding to MR and/or GR, which are both abundantly ex- pressed in CA1 pyramidal neurons (de Kloet et al., 1998). Although GR activation mediates the inhibitory effects of high CORT on LTP, conditions of predominant MR activation (resting state) are associated with efficient induction of LTP (Diamond et al., 1992; Pavlides et al., 1996; Joels and Krugers, 2007). Differences in MR and GR protein levels between low and high LG offspring may thus affect LTP, both under resting and stress- like conditions. We measured protein levels for both MR and GR in the hippocampus of offspring of low and high LG mothers. In agreement with previous reports (Liu et al., 1997; Weaver et al., 2004, 2005), we observed lower GR protein levels in low LG [relative optical density (ROD), 32.85 Ϯ 1.64] compared with high LG adult offspring (ROD, 58.77 Ϯ 1.75; t ϭ 10.34; df ϭ 10; p Ͻ 0.001, unpaired t test) (Fig. 4 A ). We extended this finding by showing that MR levels are also lower in low LG (ROD, 17.8 Ϯ 1.48) compared with high LG adult offspring (ROD, 33.37 Ϯ 3.31; t ϭ 3.68; df ϭ 10; p Ͻ 0.01, unpaired t test) (Fig. 4 B ). However, calculation of the MR/GR ratio revealed no differences between low LG and high LG offspring (low LG, 0.54 Ϯ 0.05, vs high LG, 0.53 Ϯ 0.05). The MR/GR ratio was obtained by dividing the ROD value of MR by that of GR for each phenotype. Next, we tested whether the differential levels of GR and MR expression reported above are associated with functional effects of high CORT on CA1 neuronal excitability. We pretreated hippocampal slices from adult offspring of low and high LG mothers with stress-like levels of CORT (100 n M ) and induced LTP 1– 4 h later to allow gene-mediated GR effects to develop (Karst et al., 2000). We report a significant interaction between maternal care (low vs high LG) and treatment (VEH vs CORT; F (1,8) ϭ 60.31; p Ͻ ...
Context 2
... quality of parent– child interactions predicts the risk for psy- chopathology over the lifespan (Oakley Browne et al., 1995; Kendler et al., 2002; McEwen, 2003; Nemeroff, 2004a,b; Coldwell et al., 2006). Studies in the rat suggest direct effects of maternal care on the development of neural systems that regulate cognitive, emotional, and neuroendocrine responses to stress (Meaney, 2001) and thus influence vulnerability for mood disorders. In the rat, naturally occurring variations in the frequency of pup licking and grooming (LG) provided by the dam during the first week of life are associated with individual differences in stress responsiveness, emotionality, and cognitive functioning in adult offspring. As adults, offspring of low compared with high LG mothers show increased hypothalamus-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) responses to stress (Liu et al., 1997; Weaver et al., 2004, 2005), enhanced emotionality (Caldji et al., 1998; Menard and Hakvoort, 2007), and impaired performance in tests of spatial learning and object recognition (Liu et al., 2000; Bredy et al., 2003, 2004; Toki et al., 2007). These effects are essentially reversed with cross fostering, suggesting a direct effect of maternal care (Francis et al., 1999; Caldji et al., 2003). The maternal effects on behavioral and HPA responses to stress depend on, at least in part, epigenetic programming of gene expression (Meaney and Szyf, 2005; Diorio and Meaney, 2007). However, such effects, and especially those on hippocampal- dependent forms of learning and memory, may also involve differences in synaptic plasticity. Indeed, maternal care alters the expression of synaptophysin, growth factors, and selected glutamate receptor subunits (Liu et al., 2000; Bredy et al., 2003, 2004; Toki et al., 2007). However, potential effects on the structure and function of hippocampal neurons have not been examined and are of particular relevance in light of studies suggesting parental effects on hippocampal volume in humans (Vythilingam et al., 2002; Buss et al., 2007) and of consistent reports of decreased hippocampal volume in patients with mood disorders (Sheline et al., 1996; Bremner et al., 2000). In the studies reported here, we examined the effects of variation in pup LG on the morphology of CA1 hippocampal neurons and on synaptic plasticity within this region in response to corticosteroid treatments that mimic basal versus stressful conditions (i.e., examining hippocampal function under a range of environmental conditions). Specifically, we investigated effects of maternal care on the following: (1) the morphology of CA1 neurons under resting condition, (2) LTP under low and high corticosterone (CORT) conditions ( in vitro ), (3) behavioral (learning/memory) performance in a fear conditioning task (high stress, in vivo ), and (4) levels of hippocampal mineralocorticoid receptor (MR) and glucocorticoid receptor (GR) expression. The frequency of the maternal behaviors is listed in Table 1. In addition to differing in the percentage of LG averaged over the first week of life, dams used in the current study also differed in the percentage of flat-back nursing (a form of passive maternal care) with low LG dams displaying significantly more flat-back nursing than high LG dams ( t ϭ 2.78; df (30) , p Ͻ 0.01, unpaired t test). High LG dams displayed significantly more LG than low LG dams across all days of the observation period ( F (1,21) ϭ 145.11; p Ͻ 0.0001, two-way ANOVA, repeated measures) (Fig. 1 B ). Furthermore, the frequency of maternal LG decreased over days across both groups (uncorrected, F (5,105) ϭ 3.10, p Ͻ 0.05; H-F corrected, F (3.8,79.7) ϭ 3.10; ␧ ϭ 0.759; p Ͻ 0.05). These findings are in agreement with previous reports and support the view that, in general, low LG dams display a more passive maternal care style than high LG dams (Champagne et al., 2003; Menard and Hakvoort, 2007). Enduring effects of variations in maternal care on hippocampal development have been suggested to occur via mechanisms similar to those associated with experience-dependent neural development (for review, see Meaney, 2001). This raises the question of whether early sensory experiences such as maternal LG (a form of tactile stimulation) could stably alter dendritic morphology and/or spine density during a critical period of development. To address this question, we used Golgi staining, a sensitive method allowing accurate assessment of morphological aspects of hippocampal neurons. We report morphological alterations in the length of both basal ( t ϭ 3.056; df ϭ 11; p Ͻ 0.01, unpaired t test) and apical ( t ϭ 2.38; df ϭ 11; p Ͻ 0.05, unpaired t test) CA1 pyramidal cell dendrites that were significantly shorter in adult offspring of low versus high LG mothers (Fig. 2 A , Table 2). Spine density (as determined in 20 ␮ m segments) of apical ( t ϭ 2.88; df ϭ 11; p Ͻ 0.05, unpaired t test) and basal ( t ϭ 2.57; df ϭ 11; p Ͻ 0.05, unpaired t test) dendrites was also significantly lower in low versus high LG offspring (Fig. 2 B , Table 2). Although no significant differences were observed in the number of dendritic branches (data not shown) as a function of maternal care, a trend toward statistical significance for the number of branching points was observed between offspring of low and high LG mothers, with low LG offspring displaying a trend toward fewer branching points on apical ( t ϭ 1.97; df ϭ 11, p Ͼ 0.05, unpaired t test) but not basal ( t ϭ 0.91; df ϭ 11; p Ͼ 0.05, n.s. unpaired t test) dendrites than high LG offspring (Table 2). Previous reports showed that as early as P8, there is decreased expression of genes encoding NMDA receptor (NR) subunits (i.e., NR2A and NR2B, particularly in the CA1 subfield) in the offspring of low compared with high LG mothers (Liu et al., 2000; Bredy et al., 2004). These characteristics endure into adulthood and correlate with impaired cognitive performance in hippocampal-dependent tasks (e.g., Morris water maze, object recognition) (Liu et al., 2000; Bredy et al., 2003). Therefore, we induced tetanic stimulation of Schaffer collaterals in hippocampal slices taken from low and high LG rats, which were bathed with either VEH or CORT and recorded synaptic activity for the next 60 min afterward. We report a significant interaction between maternal care (low versus high LG) and treatment (VEH versus CORT; F (1,8) ϭ 60.31; p Ͻ 0.001, two-way ANOVA, repeated measure). Sidak post hoc comparisons revealed dramatic differences after tetanic stimulation as a function of maternal care ( p Ͻ 0.001). Although synaptic potentiation was very prominent in rats from high LG mothers, no significant synaptic potentiation was seen 60 min after tetanic stimulation (10 Hz; 900 pulses) in offspring from low LG mothers (Fig. 3). It is noteworthy that the maximal responses evoked by low-frequency stimulation were significantly lower in offspring of low LG versus high LG mothers ( t ϭ 3.59; df ϭ 17; p Ͻ 0.01, unpaired t test), whereas half-maximum stimulus intensity values were comparable between both groups (Table 3). These findings suggest alterations in pre- and post-LTP synaptic transmission properties between the adult offspring of high and low LG mothers and reinforce the suggestion that differential synaptic transmission might be re- lated to differences in morphological aspects of CA1 neurons reported above. To ascertain that the effects reported above were not attributable to different levels of circulating basal CORT and ACTH, trunk blood samples were taken at rest when animals were killed by decapitation for either Golgi analysis (cohort 1: low LG, n ϭ 6; high LG, n ϭ 7), glucocorticoid receptor system analysis (cohort 2: low LG, n ϭ 5; high LG, n ϭ 5), or electrophysiology (cohort 3: low LG, n ϭ 10; high LG, n ϭ 11). For all cohorts, we report that CORT and ACTH levels did not differ under resting condition between low and high LG offspring. All values for CORT and ACTH levels are reported in Table 4 for each individual cohort. Stress and high CORT are known modulators of hippocampal functioning during learning and memory. CORT exerts its action via binding to MR and/or GR, which are both abundantly ex- pressed in CA1 pyramidal neurons (de Kloet et al., 1998). Although GR activation mediates the inhibitory effects of high CORT on LTP, conditions of predominant MR activation (resting state) are associated with efficient induction of LTP (Diamond et al., 1992; Pavlides et al., 1996; Joels and Krugers, 2007). Differences in MR and GR protein levels between low and high LG offspring may thus affect LTP, both under resting and stress- like conditions. We measured protein levels for both MR and GR in the hippocampus of offspring of low and high LG mothers. In agreement with previous reports (Liu et al., 1997; Weaver et al., 2004, 2005), we observed lower GR protein levels in low LG [relative optical density (ROD), 32.85 Ϯ 1.64] compared with high LG adult offspring (ROD, 58.77 Ϯ 1.75; t ϭ 10.34; df ϭ 10; p Ͻ 0.001, unpaired t test) (Fig. 4 A ). We extended this finding by showing that MR levels are also lower in low LG (ROD, 17.8 Ϯ 1.48) compared with high LG adult offspring (ROD, 33.37 Ϯ 3.31; t ϭ 3.68; df ϭ 10; p Ͻ 0.01, unpaired t test) (Fig. 4 B ). However, calculation of the MR/GR ratio revealed no differences between low LG and high LG offspring (low LG, 0.54 Ϯ 0.05, vs high LG, 0.53 Ϯ 0.05). The MR/GR ratio was obtained by dividing the ROD value of MR by that of GR for each phenotype. Next, we tested whether the differential levels of GR and MR expression reported above are associated with functional effects of high CORT on CA1 neuronal excitability. We pretreated hippocampal slices from adult offspring of low and high LG mothers with ...

Citations

... These findings were consistent with results from rodent studies showing that resource scarcity was associated with structural deficits in the hippocampus, including dendritic atrophy and apoptosis. [19][20][21][22][23] The ADI has been operationalized as a composite score of various neighborhood-level adverse characteristics, including general socioeconomic status (ie, poverty) and social fragmentation (ie, percentage of single-parent households). 14 Single-parent households have been shown to be associated with financial hardship and poor social networks, which may lead to disruption of social ties or lack of connections between community members. ...
Article
Importance Area deprivation index (ADI) has been shown to be associated with reduced hippocampal volume (HV) among youths. The social environment may interact with the association between ADI and HV. Objective To investigate which aspects of ADI are uniquely associated with bilateral HV and whether school and family environments have moderating interactions in associations between ADI and HV. Design, Setting, and Participants This cross-sectional study used data from the Adolescent Brain and Cognitive Development (ABCD) study. Participants aged 9 and 10 years were recruited from 21 sites in the US between September 2016 and August 2018. Data analysis was performed between March 2023 and April 2024. Exposures ADI aspects were derived from participant primary home addresses provided by parents or guardians. Main Outcomes and Measures HV was automatically segmented from structural brain images ascertained from magnetic resonance imaging. Multiple generalized linear mixed modeling tested associations between 9 indices of ADI and bilateral HV, with family groups and recruitment sites as random effects. After stepwise backward selection, models were adjusted for individual-level covariates, including age, sex, race and ethnicity, parental education, household income, and estimated intracranial volume. Results This study included 10 114 participants aged 9 and 10 years (median [IQR] age, 9.92 [9.33-10.48] years; 5294 male [52.3%]; 200 Asian [2.0%], 1411 Black [14.0%], and 6655 White [65.8%]; 1959 Hispanic [19.4%]). After stepwise backward selection and adjusting for covariates, only the percentage of neighborhood-level single-parent households was associated with right HV (adjusted β per 1-SD increase in single-parent households, −0.03; 95% CI, −0.06 to −0.01; P = .01). School environment interacted with neighborhood-level single-parent households in its association with right HV (adjusted β per 1-SD increase in score, 0.02; 95% CI, 0.01 to 0.03; P = .003), such that there was an inverse association only among those at a school with the mean environment score (adjusted β per 1% increase in single-parent households, −0.03; 95% CI, −0.05 to −0.01; P = .02) and worse (−1 SD score) school environment score (adjusted β per 1% increase in single-parent households, −0.05; 95% CI, −0.09 to −0.01; P < .001) but not among those at better (+1 SD score) school environments. Conclusions and Relevance In this study, an increased percentage of neighborhood-level single-parent households was associated with reduced right HV among children in schools with the mean or worse but not better environment score. These findings suggest that longitudinal research concerning the association of neighborhood-level characteristics and school environments with hippocampal development may be warranted to better understand complex interactions between various social factors and child neurodevelopment and mental health outcomes.
... As a prevailing external factor, stress has both immediate and long-lasting effects on cognitive functions (Kim and Diamond, 2002;Brunson et al., 2005;Champagne et al., 2008;Sandi, 2013). Previous studies indicate that stress experience during the postnatal period (Brunson et al., 2005;Kosten et al., 2006Kosten et al., , 2007 and the mid-adulthood period (Sandi and Touyarot, 2006;Borcel et al., 2008) can lead to cognitive impairment later in aged animals. ...
... Previous research suggests a potential neurobiological mechanism underlying the observed impairment in RM and EM in EAS aged rats (Barnes, 1988;Geinisman et al., 1992;Kim and Diamond, 2002;Aleisa et al., 2006;Champagne et al., 2008;Arias-Cavieres et al., 2017;Dulka et al., 2020). These findings are consistent with the detrimental changes that occur in the hippocampal system due to stress and aging. ...
Article
Full-text available
Cognitive aging widely varies among individuals due to different stress experiences throughout the lifespan and vulnerability of neurocognitive mechanisms. To understand the heterogeneity of cognitive aging, we investigated the effect of early adulthood stress (EAS) on three different hippocampus-dependent memory tasks: the novel object recognition test (assessing recognition memory: RM), the paired association test (assessing episodic-like memory: EM), and trace fear conditioning (assessing trace memory: TM). Two-month-old rats were exposed to chronic mild stress for 6 weeks and underwent behavioral testing either 2 weeks or 20 months later. The results show that stress and aging impaired different types of memory tasks to varying degrees. RM is affected by combined effect of stress and aging. EM became less precise in EAS animals. TM, especially the contextual memory, showed impairment in aging although EAS attenuated the aging effect, perhaps due to its engagement in emotional memory systems. To further explore the neural underpinnings of these multi-faceted effects, we measured long-term potentiation (LTP), neural density, and synaptic density in the dentate gyrus (DG). Both stress and aging reduced LTP. Additionally, the synaptic density per neuron showed a further reduction in the stress aged group. In summary, EAS modulates different forms of memory functions perhaps due to their substantial or partial dependence on the functional integrity of the hippocampus. The current results suggest that lasting alterations in hippocampal circuits following EAS could potentially generate remote effects on individual variability in cognitive aging, as demonstrated by performance in multiple types of memory.
... Via these actions, GCs regulate synaptic function (Popoli et al., 2012;Sanacora et al., 2022) and memory processes including memory consolidation, memory retrieval and memory generalization (Bahtiyar et al., 2020;Brosens et al., 2023a,b;dos Santos Corrêa et al., 2021;Hui, 2004;Kaouane et al., 2012;Lesuis et al., 2021;McReynolds et al., 2010;Quirarte et al., 2009). While cellular studies suggest that ELS increases the sensitivity of synapses to GCs (Champagne et al., 2008;Pillai et al., 2018) relatively little is known whether/how ELS alters the sensitivity to glucocorticoids at the behavioral level. ...
... For example, adult offspring of high LG dams exhibit increased hippocampal synaptogenesis and synaptic density from preweaning through adulthood. 151,158 Within the hippocampus of communally reared offspring, the rates of survival of newly generated neurons are elevated, which is consistent with the established role of BDNF in supporting cell survival. 159 Other forms of enrichment during development also induce dendritic arborization and increases in synapse number and size throughout the cortex. ...
Article
Full-text available
Early life experiences can have an enduring impact on the brain and behavior, with implications for stress reactivity, cognition, and social behavior. In particular, the neural systems that contribute to the expression of social behavior are altered by early life social environments. However, paradigms that have been used to alter the social environment during development have typically focused on exposure to stress, adversity, and deprivation of species‐typical social stimulation. Here, we explore whether complex social environments can shape the development of complex social behavior. We describe lab‐based paradigms for studying early life social complexity in rodents that are generally focused on enriching the social and sensory experiences of the neonatal and juvenile periods of development. The impact of these experiences on social behavior and neuroplasticity is highlighted. Finally, we discuss the degree to which our current approaches for studying social behavior outcomes give insight into “complex” social behavior and how social complexity can be better integrated into lab‐based methodologies.
... The environmental stressors linked to increased activity of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis in rodent studies have predominately involved low maternal nurturance or unpredictability, indexed by variations in responsiveness of maternal care (Drury et al., 2016;Turecki and Meaney, 2016). Low maternal responsiveness is associated with a host of alterations in the development of rodent pups, including changes in HPA axis output, circuitry of the amygdala and hippocampus, and behavioral functioning that persist into adulthood (e. g., Champagne et al., 2008;Drury et al., 2016;Turecki and Meaney, 2016). For example, Champagne and colleagues (2008) found that high corticosterone administration was associated with greater long-term potentiation in the hippocampus for pups exposed to lower maternal licking and grooming, potentially reflecting enhanced hippocampal plasticity under high-stress conditions. ...
... Among rodents, controlled studies have demonstrated that higher early HPA axis activation is related to altered amygdala and hippocampal structure and function (Maras and Baram, 2012;Vyas et al., 2004;Vyas et al., 2006). In addition, low maternal nurturance/maternal unpredictability is a potent stressor associated with altered neurobiological development in rodent pups (Champagne et al., 2008;Drury et al., 2016;Turecki and Meaney, 2016). To our knowledge, no work to date has assessed whether similar relations are seen among humans during the first two years of life when limbic structures are most rapidly developing (Gilmore et al., 2012). ...
Article
Full-text available
Despite a large animal literature documenting the role of low maternal nurturance and elevated glucocorticoid production on offspring limbic development, these pathways have not yet been assessed during human infancy. Informed by animal models, the present study examined whether 1) maternal disrupted interaction is related to infant cortisol levels, 2) infant cortisol levels are associated with infant limbic volumes, and 3) infant cortisol levels mediate associations between maternal disrupted interaction and infant limbic volumes. Participants included 57 mother-infant dyads. Infant saliva was measured at one time point before and two time points after the Still-Face Paradigm (SFP) at age 4 months. Five aspects of maternal disrupted interaction were coded during the SFP reunion episode. Between 4 and 25 months (M age = 11.74 months, SD = 6.12), under natural sleep, infants completed an MRI. Amygdala and hippocampal volumes were calculated via automated segmentation. Results indicated that 1) maternal disrupted interaction, and specifically disoriented interaction, with the infant was associated with higher infant salivary cortisol (AUCg) levels during the SFP, 2) higher infant AUCg was related to enlarged bilateral amygdala and hippocampal volumes, and 3) infant AUCg mediated the relation between maternal disrupted interaction and infant amygdala and hippocampal volumes. Findings are consistent with controlled animal studies and provide evidence of a link between increased cortisol levels and enlarged limbic volumes in human infants. Results further suggest that established interventions to decrease maternal disrupted interaction could impact both infant cortisol levels and infant limbic volumes.
... Diverse results were also obtained when pups received relatively poor maternal care (poor licking/cleaning), causing impaired spatial learning (Liu et al. 2000) but also improved memory for stressful events-known as contextual conditioning- (Champagne et al. 2008;Bagot et al. 2009). Similarly, pups deprived of maternal care showed impaired spatial learning in the water maze (Oomen et al. 2010), but improved fear-conditioned memory (Oomen et al. 2011). ...
Article
Full-text available
One of the main selection pressures to which animals are exposed in nature is predation, which affects a wide variety of biological traits. When the mother experiences this stressor during pregnancy and/or lactation, behavioral and physiological responses may be triggered in the offspring as well. Thus, in order to broaden and deepen knowledge on the transgenerational effects of predation stress, we evaluated how maternal stress experienced during pregnancy and/or lactation affects the spatial abilities of progeny at the onset of adulthood in the subterranean rodent Ctenomys talarum. The results showed that, contrary to what was observed in other rodent species, maternal exposure to predator cues during pregnancy and lactation did not negatively affect the spatial abilities of the offspring, even registering some minor positive effects. Concomitantly, no effects of predatory cues on physiological parameters associated with stress were observed in the progeny. This difference in results between the present study and previous works on maternal stress highlights the importance of considering the species to be evaluated (strain, age and origin—wild or captive—) and the type of stressor used (artificial or natural, intensity of exposure) in the evaluation of the possible transgenerational effects of maternal stress.
... This suggests that touch with CT optimal velocities is suited to change an infant's interoceptive state and to promote homeostasis (Püschel et al., 2022). Additionally, there is some indication that gentle touch may be relevant for promoting pro-social processes and the developing social brain with causal evidence from non-human animals (Champagne et al., 2008;Simpson et al., 2019) and correlational evidence from humans (Brauer et al., 2016;Della Longa et al., 2019;Reece et al., 2016). ...
Article
Full-text available
Since their initial discovery in cats, low-threshold C-fiber mechanoreceptors have become a central interest of scientists studying the affective aspects of touch. Their pursuit in humans, here termed C-tactile (CT) afferents, has led to the establishment of a research field referred to as "affective touch", which is differentiated from "discriminative touch". Presently, we review these developments based on an automated semantic analysis of more than 1000 published abstracts as well as empirical evidence and the solicited opinions of leading experts in the field. Our review provides a historical perspective and update of CT research, it reflects on the meaning of "affective touch", and discusses how current insights challenge established views on the relation between CTs and affective touch. We conclude that CTs support gentle, affective touch, but that not every affective touch experience relies on CTs or must necessarily be pleasant. Moreover, we speculate that currently underappreciated aspects of CT signaling will prove relevant for the manner in which these unique fibers support how humans connect both physically and emotionally.
... Pregnant rats were stressed by placing their cages on an elevated platform for a total of 20 min per day during gestational days 12-16 and this environment was sufficiently stressful to show substantial gene regulation changes via microarray analysis, with 200 dysregulated genes in the female hippocampus compared to 167 in the males. Rat pups are not born fully developed and maternal care during early life has also been implicated in hippocampal plasticity, gene methylation, and gene expression in offspring aged 7-17 weeks [85][86][87][88][89][90]. ...
Article
Full-text available
For decades, the Barker hypothesis and thrifty phenotype hypothesis have driven researchers to explore the development of metabolic syndrome through fetal programming. In this short review, we provide peer-reviewed support for the fetal programming of neural genetic activity and behavior in multiple neural regions: the prefrontal cortex, the cerebral cortex, the hippocampus, the cerebellum, and the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis. We also introduce ionizing radiation as a purported indirect driver of phenotypical changes. The predisposition of brain and behavioral phenotypes after gestational exposure to stressors can lead to aversive and harmful outcomes, rather than protective adaptations.
... In contrast, offspring of low-caring mothers show later in life more enhanced HPA-axis-and emotional reactivity than intensely nursed pups [151]. Neglected pups have, as adults, still atrophied hippocampal pyramidal neurons and epigenetically downregulated MR and GR expression, while contextual memory performance is impaired and neurogenesis suppressed [152]. These animals show increased social avoidance, but better performance in fear-motivated behavior as underscored by altered hippocampal and amygdala plasticity [153,154]. ...
... These animals show increased social avoidance, but better performance in fear-motivated behavior as underscored by altered hippocampal and amygdala plasticity [153,154]. Accordingly, experience in early life prepares for life ahead, as summarized in the match-mismatch or predictive adaptive capacity hypothesis [152,155]. ...
Article
Full-text available
In concert with neuropeptides and transmitters, the end products of the hypothalamus-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis, the glucocorticoid hormones cortisol and corticosterone (CORT), promote resilience: i.e., the ability to cope with threats, adversity, and trauma. To exert this protective action, CORT activates mineralocorticoid receptors (MR) and glucocorticoid receptors (GR) that operate in a complementary manner -as an on/off switch- to coordinate circadian events, stress-coping, and adaptation. The evolutionary older limbic MR facilitates contextual memory retrieval and supports an on-switch in the selection of stress-coping styles at a low cost. The rise in circulating CORT concentration after stress subsequently activates a GR-mediated off-switch underlying recovery of homeostasis by providing the energy for restraining the primary stress reactions and promoting cognitive control over emotional reactivity. GR activation facilitates contextual memory storage of the experience to enable future stress-coping. Such complementary MR-GR-mediated actions involve rapid non-genomic and slower gene-mediated mechanisms; they are time-dependent, conditional, and sexually dimorphic, and depend on genetic background and prior experience. If coping fails, GR activation impairs cognitive control and promotes emotional arousal which eventually may compromise resilience. Such breakdown of resilience involves a transition to a chronic stress construct, where information processing is crashed; it leads to an imbalanced MR-GR switch and hence increased vulnerability. Novel MR-GR modulators are becoming available that may reset a dysregulated stress response system to reinstate the cognitive flexibility required for resilience.
... Consistent with nonhuman animal research (58,59), human studies have established associations between exposure to maltreatment (abuse, neglect) and brain structure and function in children (17,60,61). Animal models have also indicated that quality of maternal care (e.g., licking and grooming) is associated with greater dendritic length and spine density in the hippocampus and alterations in the synaptic plasticity and glucocorticoid receptor transcription in the hippocampus and amygdala (59,62,63). In human studies, higher-quality caregiving has been linked with volumes of infant limbic and prefrontal structures, connectivity of frontolimbic pathways, and activation patterns in socially relevant areas of the infant brain (64-68) and with higher frontal resting alpha and theta electroencephalography (EEG) power and frontal EEG asymmetry (64,69). ...
... This conclusion is complemented by translational research on animals suggesting that high-quality maternal caregiving and environmental enrichment may reverse the effects of prenatal stress on brain structure and function in offspring (22,32). For instance, prenatal stress leads to decreases in dendritic spine density and volume in the hippocampus, while postnatal environmental enrichment and high-quality care lead to increases in these measures of hippocampal morphology (32,63). Similar proximal postnatal environmental factors (parental sensitivity, cognitive stimulation) have been associated with promotive influences on brain development in humans (64,66,84,86,98,114). ...
Article
Heightened maternal stress during pregnancy is associated with atypical brain development and elevated risk for psychopathology in offspring. Supportive environments during early postnatal life may promote brain development and reverse atypical developmental trajectories induced by prenatal stress. We review studies focused on the role of key early environmental factors in moderating associations between prenatal stress exposure and infant brain and neurocognitive outcomes. Specifically, we focus on the associations between parental caregiving quality, environmental enrichment, social support, and socioeconomic status (SES) with infant brain and neurocognitive outcomes. We examine the evidence that these factors may moderate the effects of prenatal stress on the developing brain. Complementing findings from translational models, human research suggests that high-quality early postnatal environments are associated with indices of infant neurodevelopment that have also been associated with prenatal stress, such as hippocampal volume and frontolimbic connectivity. Human studies also suggest that maternal sensitivity and higher SES may attenuate the effects of prenatal stress on established neurocognitive and neuroendocrine mediators of risk for psychopathology, such as hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis functioning. Biological pathways that may underlie the effects of positive early environments on the infant brain, including the epigenome, oxytocin, inflammation, are also discussed. Future research in humans should examine resilience-promoting processes in relation to infant brain development, using large sample sizes and longitudinal designs. The findings from this review could be incorporated into clinical models of risk and resilience during the perinatal period and used to design more effective early programs that reduce risk for psychopathology.