Figure 1 - uploaded by Anne Van Kleeck
Content may be subject to copyright.
An example of employing the CHAMPs approach to classroom management to story time in a preschool or kindergarten classroom. 

An example of employing the CHAMPs approach to classroom management to story time in a preschool or kindergarten classroom. 

Source publication
Article
Full-text available
Teaching academic language has recently become a separate focus from teaching subject content for school-aged children, but it is rarely considered with preschoolers and kindergartners. The critical importance of fostering academic language before children enter elementary school has recently been posited and supported by various strands of researc...

Context in source publication

Context 1
... their opinions or likes and dislikes. So, a child can be asked to select between two or more choices of which outfit to wear to school, which book they would like the adult to read and discuss with them, what type of sandwich they would like to take to school, and so forth. Even “directives” from the adult can be couched as a choice. A child might be asked, “Which would you like to do first, put on your shirt or put on your pants?” In an intervention context, the child could be asked to choose from one of two or more activities planned for the session. Children can also be asked their opinions about all matter of things, such as asking, “Did you like the story I just read?” Requests for verbal display: Children from mainstream culture families in the U.S. are often called upon to display skills they are in the process of learning. This casts them into what anthropologists have referred to as an exhibitionist role [21]. Schools, being mainstream institutions, operate similarly regarding this exhibitionist role. More collectivist cultures expect children to learn more by observing, and cast them into more of a spectator role [22]. One particular type of display children from mainstream culture backgrounds are often called upon to engage in by adults has been referred to as verbal display [21]. That is, they are requested to answer many questions in which the goal is to show what they know, and not to provide information that is unknown to the person asking the question, as is typical in the CT register. Educators have referred to these kinds of questions in a number of ways, with examples including known information questions [23], questions with known answers [24], and test questions [25]. Van Kleeck and Schwarz [26] suggested another type of verbal display that requests that children display what they are thinking rather than what they already know. As such, they distinguished requests for verbal display of already acquired knowledge from requests for verbal display of thinking. In school [27] and often in mainstream culture homes, children are very frequently called upon to verbally display what they know. Ideally, they are also frequently called upon to verbally display what they are thinking, because their exposure to these more challenging kinds of questions predicts their later language and literacy skills [28]. Verbal display of thinking relates conceptually to what educators have referred to variously as progressive discourse [29,30], instructional conversation [31], learning talk [32,33], and accountable talk [34]. Not all children get extensive experience responding to requests for verbal display in their homes. Professionals need to be certain that children with LI, and in particular children with LI from CLD backgrounds where AT is often much less frequently used, understand what their role is when such questions are asked. This may be particularly necessary because at least a small amount of evidence indicates that children from some backgrounds may be discouraged from answering such questions [35], or they may be used to tease or chastise in their cultural groups [36,37]. Van Kleeck and Schwarz [26] came up with scripts to suggest ways in which a teacher, for example, might explain to a group of children the nature of the kinds of questions that are often posed to them in school and the protocol for responding to them. In explaining requests for verbal display of knowledge, the teacher might say something along the lines of the following: “Because we are in school, I’m going to ask you and the other children questions I already know the answer to. . . . That helps me know if I’m doing a good job teaching you. If you don’t know the answer, that’s okay, too. May be another child or I will give the answer” [26]. Page 4 of 13 To explain requests for verbal display of thinking, the teacher might say, “Sometimes, you might not know the answer to questions I ask, but you can think about what the answer might be, and you can tell us what you are thinking.” The teacher might then model such a request, and a possible response to it, and introduce this by saying, “Let me show you what I mean.” She or he could then follow up with, “I wonder who this book is going to be about? I haven’t read this book before, so I don’t know for sure who it is about. But I can use hints from the cover to guess who the book might be about.” The teacher or clinician could then pause and ask the class if they see any hints on the cover that help them guess who the book is about. Likely some children will come up with viable answers, but if not, the teacher could continue, “There is a picture of a bear and a bird on the cover, so maybe the book is about the bird or the bear, or maybe about both. But, I’d have to read the book to find out if I made a good guess or not” [26]. Nature of topic participation: The third area considered under Rules for Participation in the Interaction is topic participation. In everyday Casual Talk (CT), the participants in the interaction often equally control the topics discussed, contributions are spontaneous, and the length of contributions made by various participants tend to be more or less balanced. When the AT register is used in school, the teacher typically controls the topic, the teacher most often either elicits contributions to the topic or manages spontaneous contributions by requiring some kind of request for permission to speak by the child (typically by raising a hand), and the teacher dominates the talk by engaging in a fair amount of monologue. Effective teachers will make rules for participation in any activity in the classroom explicit to children; that is, they directly teach children how to behave in different classroom situations or activities as well as during major transitions [38-42]. Teaching children the rules that specify the teacher’s behavioral expectations sets the stage for student success [39,40,43-46]. In contrast, poor behavior management in classrooms can lead to negative academic outcomes [47,48]. Although this research on classroom management has not directly measured an impact on children’s language skills, it does demonstrate the power of explicitly teaching children the rules for participation in any given activity. One frequently used evidence-based approach to classroom management, CHAMPs, provides a set of strategies for supporting positive behavior in the classroom [49]. It might also be able to be used effectively in small group therapy sessions with children with language impairments. The acronym CHAMPs specifies five categories in which expectations for student behavior should be explicitly taught– Conversation (Can students talk to each other during this activity?), Help (How do students get the teacher’s attention and their questions answered?), Activity (What is the task or objective? What is the end product?), Movement (Can students move about during this activity?) and Participation (How do students show they are fully participating? What does work behavior look or sound like?). The subset of these rules that focus on conversation and participation provide an example of how the rules for participation in the AT register in one particular activity might be conveyed to young children. Examples of rules for all aspects of the CHAMPs acronym that might be used during classroom story time are given in Figure ...

Similar publications

Conference Paper
Full-text available
Streets are the indispensable parts all over the world, whether it is an urban or rural area. There are hence many researches conducted on various characteristics of streets. As for social and cultural aspects, streets take a role of offering a platform for a wide range of social interactions and experience. In the context of urban areas, there is...

Citations

... Equally important is the feedback teachers provide on children's responses to open-ended questions. Adult feedback is critical in developing children's background knowledge and vocabulary, and in scaffolding children's practice of using the academic language that undergirds more formal reading and writing instruction (Gibbons, 2012;van Kleeck, 2014). Furthermore, adult feedback is key in promoting a responsive classroom climate (Barnes & Dickinson, 2017a;Barnes & Dickinson, 2017b;Piasta et al., 2012). ...
Article
Conversations between an adult and a child are effective ways to promote language and vocabulary development in young children. Considerable attention has been paid to teachers asking open‐ended questions to promote conversations. However, the feedback that follows the question is also an important part of promoting back‐and‐forth dialogue, and less attention has been paid to this aspect of an exchange. Teachers' feedback uniquely encourages conversations beyond one back‐and‐forth turn, essential for promoting rich adult–child language interactions. This paper discusses the role of teacher feedback in extending conversations that encourage children to use language in meaningful ways. We review the research findings on teacher feedback and offer evidenced‐based, practical suggestions on providing feedback, meant to support teachers and children as they engage in conversations. Asking open‐ended questions is one part of meaningful conversations; feedback extends the conversation and supports children's language development.
... Book reading is a setting that may facilitate the use of an academic register or academic language (Dickinson et al., 2014). van Kleeck (2014) asserted that academic language differs from casual talk in terms of the explicitness and precision, formality, and abstractness, which are manifested in differences at the sentence and lexical levels. Casual talk serves to accomplish everyday tasks and build relationships with others. ...
... The degree of mismatch between children's language experiences prior to school entry and the language requirements of preschool classrooms "may result in children acquiring limited facility with selected language skills" (Dickinson, 2001, p. 187). In the United States, children from low-income homes (van Kleeck, 2014) or those who are learning English as a second language (Schleppegrell, 2012) may have smaller funds of academic language than their peers. In particular, those children attending Head Start classrooms, which are classrooms established for children at risk for later academic difficulties, may possess smaller funds of vocabulary (Zill et al., 2003) and have less experience with academic language. ...
... The Common Core State Standards indicate that children should begin using and learning academic language in early childhood settings and have specific goals associated with academic vocabulary (Neuman & Wright, 2015;van Lier & Walqui, 2012). Relatively few studies have examined academic language in preschool classrooms (van Kleeck, 2014), with even fewer investigating two dimensions of academic language at the sentence and lexical levels. Thus, in the present article we examine preschool teachers' academic language practices using fine-grained measurements to determine their relationship with children's end-of-year receptive vocabulary scores. ...
Article
Research Findings: This study examines lexical- and sentence-level dimensions of academic language to describe teachers’ natural use of academic language and its association with vocabulary growth in 489 at-risk 4-year-olds enrolled in Head Start preschool classrooms. Using transcripts derived from video recordings of book-reading sessions in 52 classrooms, we developed measures to assess the relationships among teachers’ use of lexical elements (amount, sophistication, and diversity of vocabulary) and utterance length (mean length of utterance in words) and children’s end-of-year receptive vocabulary scores. Hierarchical linear models indicated that children in classrooms where teachers used more lexical elements had higher end-of-year receptive vocabulary scores than those who heard less. Conversely, children in classrooms where shorter utterances were used had higher end-of-year receptive vocabulary scores. Classroom factors also played a role in children’s vocabulary scores, indicating a need to address classroom environments in addition to teachers’ language use. Practice or Policy: Results indicate that teachers should consider the unique contributions of lexical- and sentence-level elements when providing vocabulary instruction for children with below-the-mean vocabulary scores. Implications for instruction are addressed.
... To acquire this register and support successful school transitions, children are likely to benefit from exposure to descriptive talk in the toddler and preschool years. van Kleeck (2014aKleeck ( , 2014b notes that the features of the literate language register are more prevalent in the parent-child interactions of mothers with higher levels of education during shared book reading activities and that these features are interwoven into everyday conversations in these homes. Unfortunately, we cannot address differences in the quantity and quality of descriptive talk in the home language environment, nor how common toy talk sentences may be during shared book reading. ...
Article
Purpose: The current study used an intervention design to test the hypothesis that parent input sentences with diverse lexical noun phrase (NP) subjects would accelerate growth in children’s sentence diversity. Method: Child growth in third person sentence diversity was modeled from 21 to 30 months (n = 38) in conversational language samples obtained at 21, 24, 27, and 30 months. Treatment parents (n = 19) received instruction on strategies designed to increase lexical NP subjects (e.g., The baby is sleeping.). Instruction consisted of one group education session and two individual coaching sessions which took place when children were approximately 22 to 23 months of age. Results: Treatment substantially increased parents’ lexical NP subject tokens and types (ηp2 > .45) compared to controls. Children’s number of different words was a significant predictor of sentence diversity in the analyses of group treatment effects and individual input effects. Treatment condition was not a significant predictor of treatment effects on children’s sentence diversity, but parents’ lexical NP subject types was a significant predictor of children’s sentence diversity growth, even after controlling for children’s number of different words over time. Conclusions: These findings establish a link between subject diversity in parent input and children’s early grammatical growth, and the feasibility of using relatively simple strategies to alter this specific grammatical property of parent language input.
Article
Full-text available
The aim of the present study is to monitor the effectiveness of a new conception of early literacy curriculum (in force since September 2016) which provides-unlike the previous one-a wide range of purposeful literacy practices and literacy events. The study compares graduates of previous early childhood literacy curriculum (tested in June 2016) with children who attended kindergarten in the years 2016-2019. Several indications of early literacy development were selected especially those that are culturally more sensitive, i.e. are significantly shaped by the social environment offering the broad spectrum of reading experiences. Results show statistically significant relationship of story listening comprehension with comprehension monitoring and narrative production, especially at the level of understanding the implicit meaning. The most profound group differences were found again in narratives and implicit meaning comprehension. On the other hand, no differences were found in indicators of phonemic awareness, comprehension monitoring and understanding the explicit meaning. The results are discussed in terms of their implications to educational practice.